back to index

Dr. David Anderson: The Biology of Aggression, Mating, & Arousal | Huberman Lab Podcast #89



link |
00:00:00.000
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
link |
00:00:02.280
where we discuss science and science-based tools
link |
00:00:04.880
for everyday life.
link |
00:00:09.280
I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology
link |
00:00:12.000
and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine.
link |
00:00:15.020
Today, my guest is Dr. David Anderson.
link |
00:00:17.440
Dr. Anderson is a professor of biology
link |
00:00:19.400
at the California Institute of Technology,
link |
00:00:21.960
often commonly referred to as Caltech University.
link |
00:00:25.440
Dr. Anderson's research focuses on emotions
link |
00:00:28.080
and states of mind and body.
link |
00:00:30.000
And indeed, he emphasizes how emotions
link |
00:00:31.840
like happiness, sadness, anger, and so on
link |
00:00:34.560
are actually subcategories
link |
00:00:36.280
of what are generally governed by states,
link |
00:00:39.360
that is, things that are occurring in the nervous system
link |
00:00:42.120
in our brain and in the connections between brain and body
link |
00:00:45.760
that dictate whether or not we feel good
link |
00:00:47.500
about how we are feeling and that drive our behaviors,
link |
00:00:50.660
that is, bias us to be in action or inaction
link |
00:00:54.520
and strongly influence the way we interpret our experience
link |
00:00:57.500
and our surroundings.
link |
00:00:58.680
Today, Dr. Anderson teaches us, for instance,
link |
00:01:01.160
why people become aggressive
link |
00:01:02.680
and why that aggression can sometimes take the form of rage.
link |
00:01:06.900
I also talk about sexual behavior
link |
00:01:08.900
and the boundaries and overlap
link |
00:01:11.260
between aggression and sexual behavior.
link |
00:01:13.640
And that discussion about aggression and sexual behavior
link |
00:01:16.600
also starts to focus on particular aspects
link |
00:01:19.120
of neural circuits and states of mind and body
link |
00:01:21.360
that govern things like, for instance, male-male aggression
link |
00:01:24.800
versus male-female aggression versus female-female aggression.
link |
00:01:28.760
So today, you will learn a lot
link |
00:01:30.040
about the biological mechanisms
link |
00:01:31.480
that govern why we feel the way we feel.
link |
00:01:33.880
Indeed, Dr. Anderson is an author
link |
00:01:35.840
of a terrific new popular book entitled
link |
00:01:38.320
"'The Nature of the Beast, How Emotions Guide Us.'"
link |
00:01:41.000
I've read this book several times now.
link |
00:01:43.200
I can tell you it contains so many gems
link |
00:01:45.040
that are firmly grounded in the scientific research.
link |
00:01:47.520
In fact, a lot of what's in the book
link |
00:01:49.520
contrasts with many of the common myths
link |
00:01:51.660
about emotions and biology.
link |
00:01:53.900
So whether or not you're a therapist or you're a biologist
link |
00:01:56.520
or you're simply just somebody interested
link |
00:01:58.500
in why we feel the way we feel
link |
00:02:00.200
and why we act the way we act,
link |
00:02:02.120
I cannot recommend the book highly enough.
link |
00:02:03.920
Again, the title is,
link |
00:02:04.740
"'The Nature of the Beast, How Emotions Guide Us.'"
link |
00:02:07.220
Today's discussion also ventures into topics
link |
00:02:09.480
such as mental health and mental illness.
link |
00:02:12.120
And some of the exciting discoveries
link |
00:02:13.940
that have been made by Dr. Anderson's laboratory
link |
00:02:15.760
and other laboratories identifying specific peptides,
link |
00:02:18.600
that is, small proteins that can govern
link |
00:02:21.200
whether or not people feel anxious or less anxious,
link |
00:02:23.680
aggressive or less aggressive.
link |
00:02:25.360
This is an important area of research
link |
00:02:27.140
that has direct implications
link |
00:02:28.480
for much of what we read about in the news,
link |
00:02:30.560
both unfortunate and fortunate events,
link |
00:02:33.080
and that will no doubt drive
link |
00:02:35.080
the future of mental health treatments.
link |
00:02:37.060
Dr. Anderson is considered one of the most pioneering
link |
00:02:40.600
and important researchers in neurobiology of our time.
link |
00:02:43.840
Indeed, he is a member of the National Academy of Sciences
link |
00:02:45.940
and a Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigator.
link |
00:02:49.120
I've mentioned the HHMI once or twice before
link |
00:02:51.440
when we've had other HHMI guests on this podcast,
link |
00:02:54.440
but for those of you that are not familiar,
link |
00:02:55.880
the Howard Hughes Medical Institute
link |
00:02:57.200
funds a small number of investigators
link |
00:02:59.380
doing particularly high-risk, high-benefit work,
link |
00:03:02.800
and it is an extremely competitive process
link |
00:03:05.580
to identify those Howard Hughes investigators.
link |
00:03:08.800
They're essentially appointed,
link |
00:03:09.760
and then every five years,
link |
00:03:11.540
they have to compete against one another
link |
00:03:13.720
and against a new incoming flock
link |
00:03:16.080
of would-be HHMI investigators
link |
00:03:18.680
to get another five years of funding.
link |
00:03:19.920
They are literally given a grade every five years
link |
00:03:22.100
as to whether or not they can continue, not continue,
link |
00:03:24.580
or whether or not they should worry about being funded
link |
00:03:27.120
for an extended period of time.
link |
00:03:28.540
Dr. Anderson has been an investigator
link |
00:03:30.300
with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute since 1989.
link |
00:03:33.860
I'm pleased to announce that the Huberman Lab Podcast
link |
00:03:35.740
is now partnered with Momentus Supplements.
link |
00:03:38.080
We partnered with Momentus for several important reasons.
link |
00:03:40.280
First of all, they ship internationally
link |
00:03:42.000
because we know that many of you are located
link |
00:03:43.900
outside of the United States.
link |
00:03:45.400
Second of all, and perhaps most important,
link |
00:03:47.420
the quality of their supplements is second to none,
link |
00:03:49.960
both in terms of purity and precision
link |
00:03:51.640
of the amounts of the ingredients.
link |
00:03:53.580
Third, we've really emphasized supplements
link |
00:03:56.200
that are single-ingredient supplements
link |
00:03:58.400
and that are supplied in dosages
link |
00:04:00.480
that allow you to build a supplementation protocol
link |
00:04:03.440
that's optimized for cost,
link |
00:04:05.280
that's optimized for effectiveness,
link |
00:04:07.160
and that you can add things and remove things
link |
00:04:09.300
from your protocol in a way
link |
00:04:10.640
that's really systematic and scientific.
link |
00:04:12.400
If you'd like to see the supplements
link |
00:04:13.600
that we partner with Momentus on,
link |
00:04:14.920
you can go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman.
link |
00:04:18.020
There you'll see those supplements,
link |
00:04:19.160
and just keep in mind that we are constantly expanding
link |
00:04:21.600
the library of supplements available through Momentus
link |
00:04:24.320
on a regular basis.
link |
00:04:25.240
Again, that's livemomentus.com slash Huberman.
link |
00:04:27.840
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize
link |
00:04:29.560
that this podcast is separate
link |
00:04:30.840
from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
link |
00:04:33.000
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
link |
00:04:35.200
to bring zero cost to consumer information
link |
00:04:37.080
about science and science-related tools
link |
00:04:38.980
to the general public.
link |
00:04:40.240
In keeping with that theme,
link |
00:04:41.320
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
link |
00:04:44.120
Our first sponsor is Levels.
link |
00:04:45.940
Levels is a program that lets you see
link |
00:04:47.420
how different foods affect your health
link |
00:04:49.020
by giving you real-time feedback on your diet
link |
00:04:51.040
using a continuous glucose monitor.
link |
00:04:53.440
One of the most important factors governing
link |
00:04:55.080
our immediate and long-term health,
link |
00:04:56.600
and indeed our ability to think and focus clearly,
link |
00:04:59.600
is our blood glucose levels,
link |
00:05:01.160
also referred to as blood sugar.
link |
00:05:03.040
To maintain healthy energy and focus throughout the day,
link |
00:05:05.160
you want to keep your blood glucose levels steady
link |
00:05:07.120
and avoid peaks and crashes.
link |
00:05:09.500
I first started using Levels about a year ago
link |
00:05:11.740
as a way to better understand
link |
00:05:12.840
how specific foods, food combinations,
link |
00:05:15.320
exercise, and supplements impact my blood glucose levels.
link |
00:05:18.440
And it's been tremendously informative.
link |
00:05:20.200
In fact, it's changed and shaped
link |
00:05:21.700
the way that I organize my entire day.
link |
00:05:23.900
Levels even provides a simple score after you eat a meal
link |
00:05:26.600
so you can see how different foods affect you
link |
00:05:28.100
and develop a personalized nutrition program
link |
00:05:30.120
that's ideal for you.
link |
00:05:31.700
So if you're interested in learning more about Levels
link |
00:05:33.540
and trying a continuous glucose monitor yourself,
link |
00:05:35.480
you can go to levels.link.huberman.
link |
00:05:38.080
That's levels.link.huberman.
link |
00:05:41.400
Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep.
link |
00:05:44.040
Helix Sleep makes mattresses and pillows
link |
00:05:46.080
that are of the absolute highest quality.
link |
00:05:47.960
They also have some really unique features
link |
00:05:50.000
because they are customized to your unique sleep needs.
link |
00:05:53.520
I've talked over and over again on this podcast
link |
00:05:55.360
and on other podcasts,
link |
00:05:56.680
about the fact that sleep is the foundation
link |
00:05:58.840
of mental health, physical health, and performance.
link |
00:06:01.260
There's just simply no other substitute
link |
00:06:03.100
for a quality night's sleep on a regular basis.
link |
00:06:05.600
I've been sleeping on a Helix mattress
link |
00:06:07.000
for well over a year now,
link |
00:06:08.040
and it's the best sleep that I've ever had.
link |
00:06:09.960
And that's in large part
link |
00:06:11.340
because the mattress was designed for me.
link |
00:06:14.080
What you need to know, however,
link |
00:06:15.080
is what's the ideal mattress for you?
link |
00:06:16.640
And you can do that by going to Helix site.
link |
00:06:18.980
You can take their brief quiz, which will ask you,
link |
00:06:21.320
do you sleep on your side, your back, your stomach,
link |
00:06:22.960
or maybe you don't know, or maybe all three,
link |
00:06:24.580
do you tend to run hot or cold in the night?
link |
00:06:26.120
Maybe you know, maybe you don't.
link |
00:06:28.080
At the end of that short quiz,
link |
00:06:29.080
they will match you to the ideal mattress for you.
link |
00:06:30.780
I matched to the DUSK, the D-U-S-K mattress,
link |
00:06:33.120
but again, that's what I need.
link |
00:06:34.640
That's not necessarily what you need
link |
00:06:35.800
in order to get your best night's sleep.
link |
00:06:37.680
But if you're interested in upgrading your mattress,
link |
00:06:39.200
go to helixsleep.com slash Huberman,
link |
00:06:41.480
take their two-minute sleep quiz,
link |
00:06:42.720
and they'll match you to a customized mattress for you.
link |
00:06:44.560
And you'll get up to $200 off any mattress order
link |
00:06:47.820
and two free pillows.
link |
00:06:49.000
They have terrific pillows.
link |
00:06:49.880
And you get to try out that mattress
link |
00:06:51.040
for a hundred nights risk-free.
link |
00:06:52.240
They'll even pick it up for you if you don't love it,
link |
00:06:54.200
but I'm certain you will.
link |
00:06:55.400
Again, if you're interested in,
link |
00:06:56.240
you go to helixsleep.com slash Huberman
link |
00:06:58.680
for up to $200 off your mattress order and two free pillows.
link |
00:07:02.160
Today's episode is also brought to us by Element.
link |
00:07:04.640
Element is an electrolyte drink
link |
00:07:06.280
that has everything you need and nothing you don't,
link |
00:07:08.700
meaning no sugar, but it has sodium, magnesium,
link |
00:07:11.960
and potassium in the proper ratios.
link |
00:07:13.720
Sodium, magnesium, and potassium are critical
link |
00:07:15.800
for the functioning of all your cells,
link |
00:07:17.580
but of your neurons, your nerve cells in particular.
link |
00:07:20.280
Many people out there, believe it or not,
link |
00:07:22.160
need to ingest more sodium and more potassium
link |
00:07:25.060
and magnesium in the proper ratios to sodium and potassium.
link |
00:07:29.080
For those of you that are pre-hypertensive or hypertensive,
link |
00:07:31.240
you definitely don't want to crank up your sodium intake,
link |
00:07:33.100
but many people out there,
link |
00:07:33.980
especially people following low carb diets
link |
00:07:36.400
or eating exceptionally clean,
link |
00:07:38.480
very well may need more sodium.
link |
00:07:40.900
And by drinking Element mixed with water,
link |
00:07:42.800
which is what I do once or twice a day,
link |
00:07:44.360
and certainly also during workouts and after workouts,
link |
00:07:47.400
you can optimize mental functioning
link |
00:07:49.140
and physical performance
link |
00:07:50.640
by increasing your electrolyte balance
link |
00:07:52.600
in the proper ratios.
link |
00:07:54.700
If you'd like to try Element,
link |
00:07:55.680
you can go to drink element, that's lmnt.com slash Huberman,
link |
00:07:59.920
and you'll get a free sample pack with your order.
link |
00:08:01.940
Again, that's drinklmnt.com slash Huberman
link |
00:08:05.300
to get a free sample pack with your order.
link |
00:08:07.400
And now for my discussion with Dr. David Anderson.
link |
00:08:10.760
David, great to be here
link |
00:08:11.840
and great to finally sit down and chat with you.
link |
00:08:14.120
Great to be here too.
link |
00:08:14.960
Thank you so much.
link |
00:08:16.000
Yeah, I have a ton of questions,
link |
00:08:17.420
but I want to start with something fairly basic,
link |
00:08:20.280
but that I'm aware is a pretty vast landscape.
link |
00:08:23.640
And that's the difference between emotions and states,
link |
00:08:27.680
if indeed there is a difference,
link |
00:08:28.960
and how we should think about emotions.
link |
00:08:31.880
What are they?
link |
00:08:33.560
They have all these names, happiness, sadness, depression,
link |
00:08:37.200
anger, rage.
link |
00:08:38.740
How should we think about them?
link |
00:08:39.960
And why might states be at least as useful
link |
00:08:44.520
a thing to think about, if not more useful?
link |
00:08:46.960
That's great.
link |
00:08:48.400
First, the short answer to your question
link |
00:08:50.400
is that I see emotions as a type of internal state,
link |
00:08:55.480
in the sense that arousal is also a type of internal state,
link |
00:08:59.200
motivation's a type of internal state,
link |
00:09:01.320
sleep is a type of internal state.
link |
00:09:04.240
And the sort of simplest way I think of internal states
link |
00:09:08.480
is that, as you've shown in your own work,
link |
00:09:10.500
they change the input to output transformation of the brain.
link |
00:09:15.360
When you're asleep, you don't hear something
link |
00:09:18.240
that you would hear if you were awake,
link |
00:09:20.120
unless it's a really, really loud noise.
link |
00:09:22.240
So from that broad perspective,
link |
00:09:25.040
I see emotion as a class of state that controls behavior.
link |
00:09:30.220
The reason I think it's useful to think about it as a state
link |
00:09:33.800
is it puts the focus on it as a neurobiological process,
link |
00:09:39.320
rather than as a psychological process.
link |
00:09:42.600
And this gets around all of the definitional problems
link |
00:09:46.480
that people have with the word emotion,
link |
00:09:49.560
where many people equate emotion with feeling,
link |
00:09:53.440
which is a subjective sense that we can only study in humans
link |
00:09:59.040
because to find out what someone's feeling,
link |
00:10:01.720
you have to ask them, and people are the only animals
link |
00:10:04.880
that can talk that we can understand.
link |
00:10:07.280
So that's how I think about emotion.
link |
00:10:10.560
It's the, if you think of an iceberg,
link |
00:10:13.420
it's the heart of the iceberg
link |
00:10:15.600
that's below the surface of the water.
link |
00:10:18.080
The feeling part is the tip that's sort of floating
link |
00:10:21.320
above the surface of your consciousness.
link |
00:10:23.720
Not that that isn't important, it is,
link |
00:10:25.780
but you have to understand consciousness
link |
00:10:28.300
if you wanna understand feelings,
link |
00:10:30.400
and we're not ready to study that in animals yet.
link |
00:10:33.840
And so that's how I think about it.
link |
00:10:36.480
What are the different components of a state?
link |
00:10:40.160
You know, you mentioned arousal as a key component.
link |
00:10:43.620
What are some of the other features of states
link |
00:10:45.840
that represent this, as you so beautifully put in your book,
link |
00:10:50.280
that represent below the tip of the iceberg?
link |
00:10:52.320
Right, right.
link |
00:10:53.280
So you can break states up
link |
00:10:58.280
into different facets, or people would call them dimensions.
link |
00:11:03.880
And so there have been people who've thought of emotions
link |
00:11:08.280
as having just really two dimensions,
link |
00:11:11.080
an arousal dimension, how intense is it,
link |
00:11:16.720
and also a valence dimension,
link |
00:11:19.960
which is, is it positive or negative, good or bad?
link |
00:11:24.160
Ralph Adolphs and I have tried to expand that a little bit
link |
00:11:28.360
to think about components of emotion,
link |
00:11:31.040
particularly those that distinguish emotion states
link |
00:11:34.120
from motivational states,
link |
00:11:36.120
because they are very closely related.
link |
00:11:39.640
One of those important properties is persistence.
link |
00:11:43.600
And this is something that distinguishes
link |
00:11:46.540
state-driven behaviors from simple reflexes.
link |
00:11:50.360
Reflexes tend to terminate when the stimulus turns off,
link |
00:11:55.800
like the doctor hitting your knee with a hammer.
link |
00:11:58.480
It initiates with the stimulus onset,
link |
00:12:00.920
and it terminates with the stimulus offset.
link |
00:12:03.700
Emotions tend to outlast, often,
link |
00:12:06.240
the stimulus that evoke them.
link |
00:12:08.400
If you're walking along a trail here in Southern California,
link |
00:12:12.540
you hear a rattlesnake rattling.
link |
00:12:15.200
You're gonna jump in the air,
link |
00:12:16.940
but your heart is gonna continue to beat,
link |
00:12:19.280
and your palms sweat,
link |
00:12:20.760
and your mouth is gonna be dry for a while
link |
00:12:23.320
after it's slithered off in the bush,
link |
00:12:25.480
and you're gonna be hypervigilant.
link |
00:12:27.240
If you see something that even remotely
link |
00:12:29.520
looks snake-like, a stick, you're gonna stop and jump.
link |
00:12:33.520
So persistence is an important feature of emotion states.
link |
00:12:39.520
Not all states have persistence.
link |
00:12:42.480
So for example, you think about hunger.
link |
00:12:45.420
Once you've eaten, the state is gone.
link |
00:12:48.480
You're not hungry anymore.
link |
00:12:50.120
But if you're really angry,
link |
00:12:52.380
and you get into a fight with somebody,
link |
00:12:54.280
even after the fight is over,
link |
00:12:56.120
you may remain riled up for a long time,
link |
00:13:00.080
and it takes you a while to calm down.
link |
00:13:02.440
And that may have to do with the arousal dimension
link |
00:13:05.360
or some other part of it.
link |
00:13:07.520
And then generalization is an important component
link |
00:13:11.920
of emotion states that make them,
link |
00:13:16.920
if they have been triggered in one situation,
link |
00:13:21.380
they can apply to another situation.
link |
00:13:24.400
And my favorite example of that is you come home from work
link |
00:13:28.120
and your kid is screaming.
link |
00:13:30.020
If you had a good day at work,
link |
00:13:31.600
you might pick it up and soothe it.
link |
00:13:34.040
And if you had a bad day at work,
link |
00:13:35.960
you might react very differently to it and scream at it.
link |
00:13:39.280
And so that's a generalization of the state
link |
00:13:42.280
that was triggered at work by something your boss said
link |
00:13:45.520
to you to a completely different interaction.
link |
00:13:48.920
And again, that's something that distinguishes
link |
00:13:51.480
emotion states from motivation states.
link |
00:13:54.460
Motivation states are really specific.
link |
00:13:57.240
Find and eat food, obtain and consume water.
link |
00:14:02.440
And they're involved in homeostatic maintenance.
link |
00:14:06.960
So states are very multifaceted,
link |
00:14:11.920
and just asking questions about how these components
link |
00:14:17.840
of states are encoded, like what makes a state persist?
link |
00:14:22.640
What gives a state a positive or a negative valence?
link |
00:14:26.320
How do you crank up or crank down the intensity of the state?
link |
00:14:30.240
It just opens up a whole bunch of questions
link |
00:14:32.860
that you can ask in the brain
link |
00:14:35.120
with the kinds of tools we have now.
link |
00:14:38.480
You mentioned arousal a few times,
link |
00:14:40.100
and you mentioned valence.
link |
00:14:42.040
Realizing that there are these other aspects of states,
link |
00:14:45.640
I'd like to just talk about arousal a little bit more
link |
00:14:47.820
and valence, because at a very basic level,
link |
00:14:50.240
it seems to me that arousal,
link |
00:14:52.400
we can be very alert and pissed off,
link |
00:14:55.920
stressed, worried, you have insomnia.
link |
00:15:01.140
We can also be very alert and be quite happy.
link |
00:15:03.480
So the valence flips.
link |
00:15:05.940
We could be very, people can be sexually aroused.
link |
00:15:08.320
People can be aroused in all sorts of ways.
link |
00:15:11.240
Is there any simple or simple-ish neurochemical signature
link |
00:15:16.480
that can flip valence?
link |
00:15:18.000
So for instance, is there any way that we can safely say
link |
00:15:20.840
that arousal with some additional dopamine release
link |
00:15:25.720
is going to be of positive valence,
link |
00:15:28.240
and arousal with very low dopamine
link |
00:15:31.640
is going to be of negative valence?
link |
00:15:33.860
I would be reluctant to say that it's a chemical flip.
link |
00:15:39.340
I would say it's more likely to be a circuit flip,
link |
00:15:43.260
different circuits being engaged,
link |
00:15:45.060
and it might be that a given neurochemical, even dopamine,
link |
00:15:49.000
is involved in both positively valenced arousal
link |
00:15:52.660
and negatively valenced arousal.
link |
00:15:55.060
That's why people think about these as different axes.
link |
00:15:59.300
So I think the interesting question that you touch on is,
link |
00:16:04.600
is arousal something that is just completely generic
link |
00:16:08.000
in the brain, or are there actually different kinds
link |
00:16:11.500
of arousal that are specific to different behaviors?
link |
00:16:15.340
And you raise the question,
link |
00:16:16.460
sexual arousal feels different from aggressive arousal,
link |
00:16:20.320
for example.
link |
00:16:21.540
And we actually published a paper on this back in 2009
link |
00:16:26.100
in Fruit Flies, where we found some evidence
link |
00:16:29.980
for two types of arousal states,
link |
00:16:32.860
one of which is sleep-wake arousal.
link |
00:16:35.680
You're more aroused when you wake up
link |
00:16:37.400
than when you're asleep, and flies show that.
link |
00:16:39.900
And the other is a startle response, an arousal response,
link |
00:16:45.220
to a mechanical stimulus, a noxious mechanical stimulus.
link |
00:16:48.820
If you puff air on flies,
link |
00:16:50.780
kind of like trying to swat the wasp away
link |
00:16:53.540
from your burger at the picnic table,
link |
00:16:55.640
they come back more and more and more vigorously.
link |
00:16:58.700
And we were able to dissect this and show
link |
00:17:01.820
that although both of those forms of arousal
link |
00:17:05.220
require dopamine, they were exerted
link |
00:17:09.340
through completely separable neural circuits in the fly.
link |
00:17:13.500
And so that really put, number one, the emphasis on,
link |
00:17:17.220
it's the circuit that determines the type of arousal,
link |
00:17:20.660
but also that arousal isn't unitary,
link |
00:17:24.140
that there are behavior-specific forms of arousal.
link |
00:17:27.420
And I think the jury is still out
link |
00:17:29.820
as to whether there is such a thing
link |
00:17:32.380
as completely generalized arousal or not.
link |
00:17:35.820
I think some people would argue there is,
link |
00:17:38.460
but I think more attention needs to be paid
link |
00:17:40.920
to this question of domain-specific
link |
00:17:43.820
or behavior-specific forms of arousal.
link |
00:17:46.660
Yeah, it's a super interesting idea,
link |
00:17:48.220
because I always thought of arousal as along a continuum,
link |
00:17:50.260
like you can either be in a panic attack
link |
00:17:52.060
at the one end of the extreme, or you can be in a coma,
link |
00:17:54.300
and then somewhere in the middle, you're alert and calm.
link |
00:17:56.740
But then this issue of valence really, as you say,
link |
00:18:00.580
presents this opportunity that really
link |
00:18:02.100
there might be multiple circuits for arousal
link |
00:18:05.100
or multiple mechanisms that would include neurochemicals
link |
00:18:08.940
as well as different neural pathways.
link |
00:18:11.260
So I'd like to talk a bit about a state,
link |
00:18:15.140
if it is indeed a state, which is aggression.
link |
00:18:18.100
Your lab's worked extensively on this.
link |
00:18:20.520
And if you would, could you highlight
link |
00:18:22.940
some of the key findings there,
link |
00:18:25.060
which brain areas that are involved,
link |
00:18:27.580
the beautiful work of Dai-Yu Lin and others in your lab,
link |
00:18:32.060
that point to the idea that indeed there are
link |
00:18:35.380
kind of switches in the brain,
link |
00:18:37.180
but that thinking of switches for aggression
link |
00:18:40.280
might be too simple.
link |
00:18:42.580
How should we think about aggression?
link |
00:18:44.400
And I'll just sort of skew the question a bit more
link |
00:18:48.000
by saying we see lots of different kinds of aggression,
link |
00:18:51.700
this terrible school shooting down in Texas recently,
link |
00:18:56.360
clearly an act that included aggression,
link |
00:18:58.320
and yet you could imagine
link |
00:19:00.180
that's a very different type of aggression
link |
00:19:01.380
than an all-out rage or a controlled aggression.
link |
00:19:05.540
There's a lot of variation there.
link |
00:19:06.700
So what are your thoughts on aggression,
link |
00:19:09.060
how it's generated the neural circuit mechanisms,
link |
00:19:11.000
and some of the variation in what we call aggression?
link |
00:19:13.840
Yeah, this is a great question, and it's a large area.
link |
00:19:17.700
I would say that the, first of all,
link |
00:19:21.340
the word aggression in my mind
link |
00:19:24.780
refers more to a description of behavior
link |
00:19:28.340
than it does to an internal state.
link |
00:19:32.380
Aggression could reflect an internal state
link |
00:19:35.300
that we would call anger in humans,
link |
00:19:38.280
or could reflect fear, or it could reflect hunger
link |
00:19:43.140
if it's predatory aggression.
link |
00:19:45.140
And so this gets at the issue that you raised
link |
00:19:47.700
of the different types of aggression that exist.
link |
00:19:51.420
The work that Dayu did when she was in my lab
link |
00:19:55.740
that really broke open the field
link |
00:19:58.740
to the application of modern genetic tools
link |
00:20:01.780
for studying circuits in mice
link |
00:20:04.020
is that she found a way to evoke aggression in mice
link |
00:20:09.020
using optogenetics to activate specific neurons
link |
00:20:15.460
in a region of the hypothalamus,
link |
00:20:18.340
the ventromedial hypothalamus, VMH,
link |
00:20:21.340
which people had been studying and looking at for decades,
link |
00:20:27.460
following first the work in Katz,
link |
00:20:32.380
the famous Nobel Prize winning work of Walter Hess,
link |
00:20:36.500
and then followed by work done by Meno Crook
link |
00:20:40.100
in the Netherlands in rats
link |
00:20:42.620
where they would stick electrical wires into the brain
link |
00:20:46.980
and send electric currents into the brain,
link |
00:20:49.760
and they could trigger a placid cat
link |
00:20:52.860
to suddenly bare its teeth, hiss,
link |
00:20:56.080
and almost strike out at the experimenter,
link |
00:20:59.420
and they could trigger rats to fight with each other.
link |
00:21:03.500
And even in Hess's original experiments,
link |
00:21:07.220
he describes two types of aggression
link |
00:21:10.020
that he evokes from Katz
link |
00:21:11.940
depending on where in the hypothalamus
link |
00:21:14.700
he puts his electrode,
link |
00:21:16.860
one of which he calls defensive rage,
link |
00:21:20.060
that's the ears laid back, teeth bared and hissing,
link |
00:21:24.380
and the other one is predatory aggression
link |
00:21:27.560
where the cat has its ears forward
link |
00:21:30.660
and it's like batting with its paw at a mouse-like object
link |
00:21:34.420
like it wants to catch it and eat it.
link |
00:21:36.800
So he already had at that stage some information
link |
00:21:40.740
about segregation in the brain
link |
00:21:42.820
of different forms of aggression.
link |
00:21:44.780
So fast forward to 2008, 2009
link |
00:21:53.000
when Dayu came to the lab
link |
00:21:54.780
and we had started working on aggression in fruit flies
link |
00:21:58.180
and I wanted to bring it into mice
link |
00:22:00.860
so that we could apply genetic tools.
link |
00:22:03.260
And we started by having Dayu,
link |
00:22:05.480
who was an electrophysiologist,
link |
00:22:08.180
just repeat the electrical stimulation
link |
00:22:11.780
of the ventromedial hypothalamus in the mouse,
link |
00:22:15.260
just like people had done in rats, in cats, in hamsters,
link |
00:22:19.340
even in monkeys.
link |
00:22:20.460
And she could not get that experiment to work
link |
00:22:23.580
over 40 different trials.
link |
00:22:26.420
It just didn't work.
link |
00:22:27.540
What she got instead was fear behaviors.
link |
00:22:31.140
She got freezing, cornering, and crouching.
link |
00:22:35.800
And finally in desperation,
link |
00:22:39.100
and we got a lot of input from Meno Crook on this,
link |
00:22:41.820
he really was mystified.
link |
00:22:43.100
Why doesn't it work in mice?
link |
00:22:44.860
We realized why there had been no paper
link |
00:22:47.720
on brain stimulated aggression in mice in 50 years
link |
00:22:52.460
because the experiment doesn't work.
link |
00:22:54.900
And the one bit of credit I can claim there
link |
00:22:59.220
is I convinced Dayu to try optogenetics
link |
00:23:03.320
because it just had sort of come into use deep in the brain
link |
00:23:09.800
from Carl Deiss-Roth and others' work.
link |
00:23:13.160
And I thought maybe because it could be directed
link |
00:23:16.180
more specifically to a region of the brain
link |
00:23:19.400
and types of cells and optogenetic stimulation,
link |
00:23:22.580
than electrical stimulation,
link |
00:23:24.700
it might work.
link |
00:23:25.540
And Dayu said, never, never gonna work.
link |
00:23:27.880
If it doesn't work with electricity,
link |
00:23:29.620
why should it work with optogenetics?
link |
00:23:32.700
And the fact is that it did work
link |
00:23:35.480
and we were able to trigger aggression in this region
link |
00:23:40.140
using optogenetic stimulation of ventromedial hypothalamus.
link |
00:23:44.020
And in retrospect, I think the reason
link |
00:23:46.620
that we were seeing all these fear behaviors
link |
00:23:50.320
is because right at the upper part,
link |
00:23:53.780
if you think of ventromedial hypothalamus
link |
00:23:56.740
like a pear sitting on the ground,
link |
00:23:59.300
the fat part of the pear near the ground
link |
00:24:02.000
is where the aggression neurons are,
link |
00:24:03.880
but the upper part of the pear has fear neurons.
link |
00:24:07.520
And it could be because it's so small in a mouse,
link |
00:24:11.120
when you inject electrical current anywhere in the pear,
link |
00:24:15.160
it flows up through the entire pear
link |
00:24:17.680
and it activates the fear circuits
link |
00:24:19.780
and those totally dominate aggression.
link |
00:24:22.800
And so that's why we were never able to see
link |
00:24:25.540
any fighting with electrical stimulation.
link |
00:24:28.020
Whereas when you use optogenetics,
link |
00:24:29.740
you confine the stimulation just to the region
link |
00:24:34.100
where you've implanted the channelrhodopsin gene
link |
00:24:38.500
into those neurons.
link |
00:24:41.180
And so fast forward from that,
link |
00:24:43.600
from a lot of work from Dayu now on her own at NYU
link |
00:24:47.820
and with her postdoc, Anna-Gret Faulkner,
link |
00:24:51.020
there's, as well as work of other people,
link |
00:24:53.580
there's evidence that the type of fighting
link |
00:24:57.380
that we elicit when we stimulate VMH
link |
00:25:01.820
is offensive aggression that is actually rewarding
link |
00:25:06.900
to male mice. They like it.
link |
00:25:08.380
They like it.
link |
00:25:09.300
Male mice will press, learn to poke their nose
link |
00:25:13.260
or press a bar to get the opportunity
link |
00:25:16.380
to beat up a subordinate male mouse.
link |
00:25:20.020
And in more recent experiments,
link |
00:25:22.260
if you activate those neurons and the mouse has a chance
link |
00:25:25.940
to be in one of two compartments in a box,
link |
00:25:29.100
they will gravitate towards the compartment
link |
00:25:31.260
where those neurons are activated.
link |
00:25:33.460
It has a positive valence.
link |
00:25:35.980
And when I went into this field and I was thinking,
link |
00:25:38.460
well, what goes on in my brain and my body when I'm furious?
link |
00:25:43.580
It certainly doesn't feel like a rewarding experience.
link |
00:25:47.500
It's not something that I would want to repeat
link |
00:25:50.180
because it feels good when I'm in that state.
link |
00:25:52.640
It doesn't feel good at all when I'm in that state.
link |
00:25:55.580
And it is still, I think, a mystery
link |
00:25:59.300
as to where that type of aggression,
link |
00:26:01.300
which is more defensive aggression,
link |
00:26:03.840
the kind of aggression you feel if you're being attacked
link |
00:26:07.100
or if you've been cheated by somebody,
link |
00:26:10.020
where that is encoded in the brain and how that works still,
link |
00:26:14.780
I think, is a very important mystery
link |
00:26:17.460
that we haven't solved.
link |
00:26:18.980
And predatory aggression there has been some progress on.
link |
00:26:22.260
So mice show predatory aggression.
link |
00:26:24.540
They use that to catch crickets that they eat,
link |
00:26:27.260
and that involves different circuits
link |
00:26:29.780
than the ventromedial hypothalamic circuits.
link |
00:26:32.780
So it's become clear that if you want to call it
link |
00:26:36.700
the state of aggressiveness is multifaceted.
link |
00:26:41.860
It depends on the type of aggression,
link |
00:26:44.580
and it involves different sorts of circuits.
link |
00:26:47.820
There is, there's a paper suggesting
link |
00:26:50.360
that there might be a final common pathway
link |
00:26:54.740
for all aggression in a region,
link |
00:26:57.780
which is one of my favorites.
link |
00:26:59.220
It's called the substantia in naminata,
link |
00:27:01.820
the substance with no name.
link |
00:27:03.780
You know, I like-
link |
00:27:04.620
Anatomists are so creative.
link |
00:27:06.340
Or the nucleus ambiguous, you know, or the zona inserta.
link |
00:27:10.260
These are places that no one can think of what they are.
link |
00:27:13.140
Anyhow, that might be a final common pathway
link |
00:27:16.080
for predatory aggression and offensive
link |
00:27:18.820
and defensive aggression.
link |
00:27:20.460
But it can be really hard to tell
link |
00:27:22.620
just from looking at a mouse fight,
link |
00:27:25.500
whether it's engaged in offensive or defensive aggression.
link |
00:27:29.660
We've tried to take that apart
link |
00:27:31.480
using machine learning analysis of behavior.
link |
00:27:34.620
But in rats, for example, it's much clearer
link |
00:27:37.940
when the animal is engaged in offensive
link |
00:27:40.100
versus defensive aggression.
link |
00:27:41.860
They direct their bites at different parts
link |
00:27:44.660
of the opponent's body.
link |
00:27:46.080
In particular-
link |
00:27:46.920
Neck versus, offensive aggression is flank directed.
link |
00:27:51.240
Defensive aggression goes for the neck,
link |
00:27:53.880
goes for the throat.
link |
00:27:55.200
I've seen some nature specials
link |
00:27:56.500
where in a very barbaric way, at least to me,
link |
00:27:59.860
it seems like hyenas will try and go after
link |
00:28:02.700
the reproductive axis.
link |
00:28:05.200
They'll go after testicles and penis.
link |
00:28:07.420
And they basically want to,
link |
00:28:08.860
it seems they want to limit future breeding potential.
link |
00:28:12.500
Or create pain.
link |
00:28:13.740
Or create pain or both.
link |
00:28:15.700
Yeah, I mean, in terms of offensive aggression
link |
00:28:18.820
and your reflection that it doesn't feel good,
link |
00:28:23.220
I mean, I can say,
link |
00:28:24.500
I know some people who really enjoy fighting.
link |
00:28:28.100
I have a relative who is a lawyer.
link |
00:28:29.460
He loves to argue and fight.
link |
00:28:32.160
I don't think of him as physically aggressive.
link |
00:28:34.740
In fact, he's not,
link |
00:28:35.580
but loves to fight and loves to prosecute
link |
00:28:38.580
and go after people and he's pretty effective at it.
link |
00:28:41.380
I have a friend, former military special operations
link |
00:28:44.500
and very calm guy,
link |
00:28:46.260
had a great career in military special operations.
link |
00:28:49.780
And he'll quite plainly say, I love to fight.
link |
00:28:52.700
It's one of my great joys.
link |
00:28:54.380
He really enjoyed his work.
link |
00:28:56.980
And also respected the other side
link |
00:28:58.840
because they offered the opportunity to test that
link |
00:29:01.820
and to experience that joy.
link |
00:29:03.060
So in a kind of bizarre way to somebody like me
link |
00:29:04.980
who I'll certainly defend my stance if I need to,
link |
00:29:08.740
but I certainly don't consider myself
link |
00:29:10.860
somebody who offensively goes after people
link |
00:29:12.960
just to go after them.
link |
00:29:13.800
There's no quote unquote dopamine hit here,
link |
00:29:17.260
acknowledging that dopamine does many things, of course.
link |
00:29:19.880
I have a couple of questions
link |
00:29:21.300
about the way you describe the circuitry.
link |
00:29:24.020
I should say the way the circuitry is arranged.
link |
00:29:26.880
And of course we don't know
link |
00:29:28.020
because we weren't consulted at the design phase,
link |
00:29:31.580
but why do you think there would be such a close positioning
link |
00:29:35.900
of neurons that can elicit
link |
00:29:38.220
such divergent states and behaviors?
link |
00:29:41.260
I mean, you're talking about this pear shaped structure
link |
00:29:43.540
where the neurons that generate fear
link |
00:29:46.060
are cheek to jowl with the neurons
link |
00:29:48.300
that generate offensive aggression of all things.
link |
00:29:51.660
It's like putting the neurons that control swallowing
link |
00:29:55.900
next to the neurons that control vomiting.
link |
00:29:57.980
It just seems to me that on the one hand,
link |
00:30:00.820
this is the way that neural circuits are often arranged.
link |
00:30:03.060
And yet to me, it's always been perplexing
link |
00:30:04.620
as to why this would be the case.
link |
00:30:06.300
Yeah, I think that is a very profound question.
link |
00:30:10.200
And I've wondered about that a lot.
link |
00:30:15.260
If you think from an evolutionary perspective,
link |
00:30:20.320
it might have been the case that defensive behaviors
link |
00:30:25.060
and fear arose before offensive aggression,
link |
00:30:31.580
because animals first and foremost
link |
00:30:33.840
have to defend themselves from predation by other animals.
link |
00:30:38.200
And maybe it's only when they're comfortable
link |
00:30:41.280
with having warded off predation and made themselves safe
link |
00:30:45.300
that they can start to think about
link |
00:30:47.920
who's gonna be the alpha male in my group here.
link |
00:30:51.900
And so it could be that if you think that brain regions
link |
00:30:56.420
and cell populations evolved by duplication
link |
00:31:00.260
and modification of preexisting cell populations,
link |
00:31:05.460
that might be the way that those regions
link |
00:31:08.700
wound up next to each other.
link |
00:31:11.640
And developmentally, they start out
link |
00:31:14.340
from a common pool of precursors
link |
00:31:16.780
that expresses the same gene,
link |
00:31:18.580
the fear neurons and the aggression neurons.
link |
00:31:21.300
And then with development,
link |
00:31:22.580
it gets shut off in the aggression neurons
link |
00:31:24.980
and maintained in the fear neurons.
link |
00:31:27.660
Now that view says, oh, it's an accident
link |
00:31:31.020
of evolution and development,
link |
00:31:32.700
but I think there must be a functional part as well.
link |
00:31:36.180
So one thing we know about offensive aggression
link |
00:31:38.900
is that strong fear shuts it down.
link |
00:31:42.540
Whereas defensive aggression, at least in rats,
link |
00:31:46.180
is actually enhanced by fear.
link |
00:31:48.780
It's one of the big differences between defensive aggression
link |
00:31:52.700
and offensive aggression.
link |
00:31:54.300
And you think about it, if you think about it,
link |
00:31:55.980
if offensive aggression is rewarding and pleasurable,
link |
00:31:59.300
if you start to get really scared,
link |
00:32:01.680
that tends to take the fun out of it.
link |
00:32:03.780
And maybe these two regions are close to each other
link |
00:32:07.140
to facilitate inhibition of aggression by the fear neurons.
link |
00:32:13.380
We know for a fact that if we deliberately stimulate
link |
00:32:16.600
those fear neurons at the top of the pair,
link |
00:32:19.380
when two animals are involved in a fight,
link |
00:32:21.500
it just stops the fight dead in its tracks
link |
00:32:24.300
and they go off into the corner and freeze.
link |
00:32:27.020
So at least hierarchically,
link |
00:32:29.080
it seems like fear is the dominant behavior
link |
00:32:32.560
over offensive aggression.
link |
00:32:34.340
And how that inhibition would work is not clear
link |
00:32:37.340
because all these neurons are pretty much excitatory.
link |
00:32:39.940
They're almost all glutamatergic.
link |
00:32:42.020
And so one of the interesting questions for the future
link |
00:32:45.740
is how exactly does fear dominate over
link |
00:32:49.300
and shut down offensive aggression in the brain?
link |
00:32:52.720
How does that work?
link |
00:32:53.700
Is it all circuitry?
link |
00:32:54.780
Are there chemicals involved?
link |
00:32:56.560
What's the mechanism and when is it called into play?
link |
00:33:00.340
But I think that's the way I tend to think about
link |
00:33:03.260
why these neurons are all mixed up together.
link |
00:33:06.420
And it's not just fight and freezing or fight and flight.
link |
00:33:10.520
There are also metabolic neurons
link |
00:33:13.100
that are mixed together in VMH as well.
link |
00:33:16.060
Controlling body-wide metabolism?
link |
00:33:18.380
Yeah, there are neurons there that respond to glucose.
link |
00:33:21.780
When glucose goes up in your bloodstream,
link |
00:33:24.540
they're activated.
link |
00:33:25.820
And VMH has a whole history in the field of obesity
link |
00:33:30.200
because if you destroy it in a rat, you get a fat rat.
link |
00:33:35.260
So the way most of the world thinks about VMH
link |
00:33:38.260
is they think about,
link |
00:33:39.220
oh, that's the thing that keeps you from getting fat.
link |
00:33:42.140
It's the anti-obesity area.
link |
00:33:44.220
But in the area of social behavior,
link |
00:33:47.220
we see it as a center for control of aggression
link |
00:33:50.740
and fear behaviors.
link |
00:33:51.860
And again, why these neurons and these functions,
link |
00:33:56.440
I like to call them the four Fs,
link |
00:33:58.420
feeding, freezing, fighting, and mating,
link |
00:34:00.820
that they all seem to be closely intermingled
link |
00:34:03.340
with each other, maybe because crosstalk between them
link |
00:34:06.500
is very important to help the animal's brain
link |
00:34:09.580
decide what behavior to prioritize
link |
00:34:12.860
and what behavior to shut down at any given moment.
link |
00:34:17.220
One of the things that we will do
link |
00:34:18.420
is link to the incredible videos of these mice
link |
00:34:22.400
that have selective stimulation of neurons in the VMH,
link |
00:34:26.500
DAUs and the other studies that you've done.
link |
00:34:28.860
Whenever I teach, I show those videos at some point
link |
00:34:33.540
with the caveats and warnings that are required
link |
00:34:36.620
when one is about to see a video of a mouse
link |
00:34:40.860
trying to mate with another mouse
link |
00:34:42.100
or mating with another mouse.
link |
00:34:43.460
And they seem both to be quite happy
link |
00:34:45.200
about the mating experience,
link |
00:34:46.580
at least as far as we know as observers of mice.
link |
00:34:50.220
And then upon stimulation of those VMH neurons,
link |
00:34:55.520
one of the mice essentially tries to kill the other mouse.
link |
00:34:58.940
And then when that stimulation is stopped,
link |
00:35:02.820
they basically go back to hanging out.
link |
00:35:04.700
They don't go right back to mating.
link |
00:35:06.180
There's some reconciliation clearly
link |
00:35:07.620
that needs to happen first, we assume.
link |
00:35:09.880
But it's just so striking.
link |
00:35:11.180
I think equally striking is the video
link |
00:35:12.760
where the mouse is alone in there with the glove.
link |
00:35:16.820
The VMH neurons are stimulated and the mouse goes into a rage
link |
00:35:20.180
it looks like it wants to kill the glove basically.
link |
00:35:23.020
So striking, I encourage people to go watch those
link |
00:35:25.560
because it really puts a tremendous amount of color
link |
00:35:29.740
on what we're describing.
link |
00:35:30.880
And it's just the idea that there are switches in the brain
link |
00:35:34.960
to me really became clear upon seeing that.
link |
00:35:37.460
One of the concepts, excuse me,
link |
00:35:39.420
one of the concepts that you've raised
link |
00:35:42.100
in your lectures before and that I think was Hess's idea
link |
00:35:45.340
is this idea of a sort of hydraulic pressure,
link |
00:35:47.580
or maybe it was Conrad, I can't speak now, excuse me,
link |
00:35:51.220
Conrad Lorenz, Martin, who talked about
link |
00:35:54.420
a kind of hydraulic pressure towards behavior.
link |
00:35:57.260
I'm fascinated by this idea of hydraulic pressure
link |
00:36:00.100
because I don't consider myself a hot tempered person,
link |
00:36:03.220
but I am familiar with the fact that when I lose my temper,
link |
00:36:06.420
it takes quite a while for me to simmer down.
link |
00:36:09.740
I can't think about anything else.
link |
00:36:11.260
I don't want to think about anything else.
link |
00:36:12.680
In fact, trying to think about anything else
link |
00:36:15.400
becomes aversive to me,
link |
00:36:17.420
which to me underscores this notion of prioritization
link |
00:36:21.940
of the different states and potentially conflicting states.
link |
00:36:26.780
What do you think funnels into this idea
link |
00:36:28.620
of hydraulic pressure toward a state?
link |
00:36:31.060
And why is it perhaps that sometimes we can be very angry
link |
00:36:36.080
and if we succeed in winning an argument,
link |
00:36:39.800
all of a sudden it will subside?
link |
00:36:41.940
Because clearly that means that there are external influences
link |
00:36:44.340
it's a complex space here that we're creating.
link |
00:36:46.180
I realize I'm creating a bit of a cloud
link |
00:36:47.700
and I'm doing it on purpose because to me,
link |
00:36:49.880
the idea of a hydraulic pressure towards a state like sleep,
link |
00:36:52.460
there's a sleep pressure, there's a pressure towards,
link |
00:36:55.180
that all makes sense, but what's involved?
link |
00:36:58.060
Is it too multifactorial to actually separate out
link |
00:37:02.100
the variables, but what's really driving hydraulic pressure
link |
00:37:06.540
toward a given state?
link |
00:37:08.060
Yeah, so a really important question.
link |
00:37:11.980
I think one way that is helpful, at least for me,
link |
00:37:15.460
to break this question apart and think about it
link |
00:37:18.420
is to distinguish homeostatic behaviors
link |
00:37:22.960
that is need-based behaviors where the pressure is built up
link |
00:37:27.500
because of a need, like I'm hungry, I need to eat,
link |
00:37:32.220
I'm thirsty, I need to drink, I'm hot,
link |
00:37:35.900
I need to get to a cold place.
link |
00:37:38.140
It's basically the thermostat model of your brain.
link |
00:37:41.680
You have a set point and then if the temperature
link |
00:37:44.020
gets too hot, you turn on the AC
link |
00:37:46.060
and if the temperature gets too cold,
link |
00:37:47.960
you turn on the heater and you put yourself
link |
00:37:49.700
back to the set point.
link |
00:37:51.140
I don't think that's how aggression works.
link |
00:37:53.760
That is, it's not that we all go around,
link |
00:37:56.780
at least subjectively, I don't go around
link |
00:37:59.080
with an accumulating need to fight,
link |
00:38:02.680
which I then look for something to,
link |
00:38:05.820
an excuse to release it.
link |
00:38:07.200
Now, maybe there are people that do that
link |
00:38:09.020
and they go out and look for bar fights to get into.
link |
00:38:12.460
Or Twitter.
link |
00:38:13.300
Yeah, or Twitter.
link |
00:38:14.220
Twitter seems to, I'm sort of half joking
link |
00:38:16.380
because Twitter seems to draw a reasonably sized crowd
link |
00:38:20.420
of people that are there for combat of some sort,
link |
00:38:24.440
even though the total intellectual power
link |
00:38:26.420
of any of their comments is about that of a cap gun.
link |
00:38:28.900
They seem to really like to fire off that cap gun.
link |
00:38:31.440
But I agree.
link |
00:38:33.700
Before we continue with today's discussion,
link |
00:38:35.940
I'd like to just briefly acknowledge our sponsor,
link |
00:38:38.040
Athletic Greens, now called AG1.
link |
00:38:41.260
Athletic Greens, AKA AG1, is an all-in-one
link |
00:38:44.300
vitamin mineral probiotic drink
link |
00:38:45.980
that also has adaptogens and digestive enzymes.
link |
00:38:49.020
I've been taking Athletic Greens since way back in 2012,
link |
00:38:51.740
so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
link |
00:38:54.180
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens
link |
00:38:55.780
and the reason I still drink Athletic Greens twice a day
link |
00:38:58.740
is that it supplies total foundational coverage
link |
00:39:01.640
of my vitamin mineral needs,
link |
00:39:03.040
and it supplies important nutrients
link |
00:39:05.540
that I need to support my gut microbiome.
link |
00:39:07.540
The gut microbiome, as many of you know,
link |
00:39:09.260
supports the immune system.
link |
00:39:10.380
It also supports the so-called gut-brain axis,
link |
00:39:12.400
which is vital for mood, for energy levels,
link |
00:39:14.780
for regulating focus,
link |
00:39:16.020
and many other features of our mental health
link |
00:39:17.860
and physical health that impact our daily performance
link |
00:39:20.180
and high performance in any endeavors
link |
00:39:21.820
we might be involved in.
link |
00:39:23.220
If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,
link |
00:39:24.600
you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman
link |
00:39:27.260
and claim a special offer.
link |
00:39:28.880
They're giving away five free travel packs
link |
00:39:30.540
plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2 with every order.
link |
00:39:34.420
And of course, vitamin D3K2 are vital
link |
00:39:36.660
for all sorts of things like hormone health
link |
00:39:38.740
and metabolic health and K2 for cardiovascular health
link |
00:39:41.140
and calcium regulation.
link |
00:39:42.320
Again, you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman
link |
00:39:45.540
to claim that special offer.
link |
00:39:46.860
So you can think of this accumulated hydraulic pressure
link |
00:39:51.860
either being based on something that you were deprived of,
link |
00:39:55.580
creating an accumulating need,
link |
00:39:57.980
or something that you want to do,
link |
00:40:00.580
building up a drive or a pressure to do that.
link |
00:40:04.940
And the natural way to think about that, at least for me,
link |
00:40:09.020
is as gradual increases in neural activity
link |
00:40:13.560
in a particular region of the brain.
link |
00:40:15.980
And so for example, in the area of the hypothalamus
link |
00:40:19.940
that controls feeding, Scott Stearnsen and others
link |
00:40:22.980
have shown that the hungrier you get,
link |
00:40:26.260
the higher the level of activity in that region
link |
00:40:29.440
in the brain.
link |
00:40:30.320
And then when you eat, boom,
link |
00:40:31.960
the activity goes right back down again.
link |
00:40:34.620
And that state is actually negatively valence.
link |
00:40:38.660
So it's like the animal, quote unquote,
link |
00:40:41.860
feels increasingly uncomfortable,
link |
00:40:44.580
just like we feel increasingly uncomfortable
link |
00:40:47.180
the hungrier we are.
link |
00:40:48.620
And then when we eat, it taps it down.
link |
00:40:51.020
But there is this increased activity.
link |
00:40:53.340
And I think in the case of aggression,
link |
00:40:55.500
our data and others show that the more strongly
link |
00:40:59.140
you drive this region of the brain optogenetically,
link |
00:41:03.680
the more of just a hair trigger you need
link |
00:41:07.300
to set the animal off to get it to fight.
link |
00:41:10.300
Now, the interesting thing is that if there is nothing
link |
00:41:13.660
for the animal to attack, it doesn't really do much
link |
00:41:17.580
when you're stimulating this region.
link |
00:41:19.700
It sort of wanders around the cage a little bit more,
link |
00:41:23.180
but it will not actually show overt attack
link |
00:41:27.740
unless you put something in front of it.
link |
00:41:29.660
And the same thing is true for the areas we've described
link |
00:41:32.340
that control mating behavior.
link |
00:41:34.500
This is what Lindsay is working on.
link |
00:41:36.420
You can stimulate those areas till you're blue in the face
link |
00:41:40.040
and the mouse just sort of wanders around.
link |
00:41:42.220
But if you put another mouse in, wham,
link |
00:41:45.060
he will try to mount that mouse.
link |
00:41:46.820
If you put a kumquat in the cage,
link |
00:41:49.060
he'll try to mount the kumquat.
link |
00:41:51.240
And so it becomes a sort of any port in the storm.
link |
00:41:54.880
So there is this idea that the drive is building up pressure
link |
00:42:00.820
that somehow needs to be released
link |
00:42:03.600
where that pressure is actually being exerted.
link |
00:42:07.700
If you accept that it's increased activity
link |
00:42:10.320
in some circuit or circuits someplace,
link |
00:42:12.940
what is it pushing up against that needs something else
link |
00:42:18.780
to sort of unplug it in the Lorentz hydraulic model?
link |
00:42:22.900
That is, you don't see the behavior
link |
00:42:25.100
until you release a valve on this bucket
link |
00:42:27.940
and let the accumulated pressure flow out.
link |
00:42:31.460
And that's one of the things we're trying to study
link |
00:42:33.980
in the context of the mating behavior as well.
link |
00:42:37.440
How does the information that there's an object
link |
00:42:40.620
in front of you come together with this drive state
link |
00:42:44.060
that is generated by stimulating these neurons
link |
00:42:47.020
in the hypothalamus to say, okay, pull the trigger and go,
link |
00:42:50.860
it's time to mate, it's time to attack.
link |
00:42:53.540
And we're just starting to get some insights into that now.
link |
00:42:56.860
Fascinating, and I should mention to people,
link |
00:42:59.700
Dr. Anderson mentioned Lindsay.
link |
00:43:01.020
Lindsay is a former graduate student of mine
link |
00:43:02.640
that's now a postdoc in David's lab.
link |
00:43:04.480
And I haven't caught up with her recently
link |
00:43:06.380
to hear about these experiments, but they sound fascinating.
link |
00:43:09.100
I would love to spend some time on this issue
link |
00:43:11.660
of why is it that a mouse won't attack nothing,
link |
00:43:15.980
but it'll attack even a glove or,
link |
00:43:19.140
and why, well, we'll only try and mate
link |
00:43:22.060
if there's another mouse to mate with.
link |
00:43:24.580
It's actually, I think fortunately for you,
link |
00:43:28.320
you're not spending a lot of time
link |
00:43:29.460
on Twitter and Instagram or YouTube,
link |
00:43:32.460
but there's this whole online community that exists now.
link |
00:43:35.460
As far as I know, it's almost exclusively young males
link |
00:43:39.880
who are obsessed with this idea.
link |
00:43:42.860
I'll just say it has a name,
link |
00:43:44.000
it's called NoFap of no masturbation
link |
00:43:46.440
as a way to maintain their motivation to go out
link |
00:43:48.520
and actually seek mates because of the ready availability
link |
00:43:52.100
of online pornography.
link |
00:43:54.180
There's probably a much larger population of young males
link |
00:43:57.340
that are never actually going out and seeking mates
link |
00:43:59.200
because they're getting porn addicted, et cetera.
link |
00:44:01.740
There's actually a serious issue that came up
link |
00:44:03.860
in our episode with Anna Lemke who wrote the book,
link |
00:44:05.820
Dopamine Nation, because the availability of pornography,
link |
00:44:09.420
there's a whole social context that's being created
link |
00:44:12.380
around this and genuine addiction.
link |
00:44:14.760
So humans are not like the mice
link |
00:44:17.020
or mice are not like the humans.
link |
00:44:18.540
Humans seem to resolve the issue on their own
link |
00:44:21.420
in ways that might actually impede seeking and finding
link |
00:44:24.340
of sexual partners and or long-term mates.
link |
00:44:26.780
So serious issue there.
link |
00:44:28.500
I raise it as a serious issue that I hear a lot about
link |
00:44:30.980
because I get asked hundreds, if not thousands
link |
00:44:33.060
of questions about this.
link |
00:44:33.900
Is there any physiological basis for what they call NoFap?
link |
00:44:36.900
And I never actually replied because there's no data,
link |
00:44:39.800
but what you're raising here is a very interesting
link |
00:44:43.060
mechanistic scenario that can,
link |
00:44:47.620
as you mentioned, is being explored.
link |
00:44:49.540
So what do we know about the internal state
link |
00:44:54.100
of a mouse whose VMH is being stimulated
link |
00:44:57.020
or a mouse whose other brain region
link |
00:45:00.680
that can stimulate the desire to mate?
link |
00:45:03.340
What do we know about the internal state of that mouse
link |
00:45:05.620
if it's just alone in the cage wandering around?
link |
00:45:07.500
Is it wandering around really wanting to mate
link |
00:45:09.540
and really wanting to fight?
link |
00:45:10.660
We of course don't know, but is its heart rate up?
link |
00:45:15.380
Is its blood pressure up?
link |
00:45:17.260
Is it wishing that there was pornography?
link |
00:45:19.900
Is it, something's going on presumably
link |
00:45:23.980
that's different than prior to that stimulation
link |
00:45:28.980
and is it arousal?
link |
00:45:31.260
And what do you think it is about the visual
link |
00:45:33.740
or olfactory perception of a conspecific
link |
00:45:36.960
that ungates this tremendous repertoire of behaviors?
link |
00:45:40.380
Right, that is the central question.
link |
00:45:43.820
I can say at least with respect to the fear neurons
link |
00:45:47.720
that sit on top of the aggression neurons,
link |
00:45:49.740
we know that when those neurons are activated
link |
00:45:53.340
optogenetically in the same way we would activate
link |
00:45:56.220
the aggression neurons that there's clearly an arousal
link |
00:46:00.020
process that's occurring.
link |
00:46:01.300
You can see the pupils dilate in the animal.
link |
00:46:04.320
There is an increase in stress hormone release
link |
00:46:07.660
into the bloodstream.
link |
00:46:09.100
We've shown that heart rate goes up.
link |
00:46:12.520
So in addition to the drive to actually freeze
link |
00:46:17.300
which is what those animals do,
link |
00:46:19.660
there is autonomic arousal and neuroendocrine activation
link |
00:46:24.660
of stress responses.
link |
00:46:26.220
And some of that is probably shared
link |
00:46:29.060
by the aggression neurons and the mating neurons
link |
00:46:32.220
although we haven't investigated it in as much detail.
link |
00:46:35.460
But I wouldn't be surprised because they project
link |
00:46:38.780
to many of the same regions that the fear neurons
link |
00:46:43.420
project to which is a interesting issue in the context
link |
00:46:47.660
to discuss later maybe in the context of why
link |
00:46:50.440
we're comfortable with mental illnesses that are based
link |
00:46:53.300
on maladaptations of fear but not mental illnesses
link |
00:46:56.660
that are based on maladaptations of aggression
link |
00:46:59.800
if they have pretty similar circuits in the brain.
link |
00:47:02.540
But that's how I would imagine there is
link |
00:47:06.300
an arousal dimension as you say.
link |
00:47:08.860
There are stress hormones that are activated.
link |
00:47:10.940
These regions, VMH projects to about 30 different regions
link |
00:47:15.620
in the brain and it gets input
link |
00:47:18.400
from about 30 different regions.
link |
00:47:20.100
So I kind of see it as both an antenna
link |
00:47:23.460
and a broadcasting center.
link |
00:47:25.180
It's like a satellite dish that takes in information
link |
00:47:28.820
from different sensory modalities, smell,
link |
00:47:32.260
maybe vision, mechanosensation and then it sort
link |
00:47:37.100
of synthesizes and integrates that into
link |
00:47:40.780
a fairly low dimensional as the computational people
link |
00:47:44.620
call it representation of this pressure to attack
link |
00:47:48.860
and it broadcasts that all over the brain
link |
00:47:51.820
to trigger all these systems that have to be brought
link |
00:47:54.820
into play if the animal is gonna engage in aggression
link |
00:47:58.540
because aggression is a very risky thing
link |
00:48:01.400
for an animal to engage in.
link |
00:48:03.060
It could wind up losing and it could wind up getting killed
link |
00:48:06.800
and so its brain constantly has to make
link |
00:48:10.340
a cost benefit analysis of whether to continue
link |
00:48:13.400
on that path or to back off as well.
link |
00:48:16.460
And I think that part of this broadcasting function
link |
00:48:19.620
of this region is engaging all these other brain domains
link |
00:48:24.620
that play a role in this kind of cost benefit analysis.
link |
00:48:29.320
I wanna talk more about mating behavior,
link |
00:48:31.380
but as a segue to that, as we're talking about aggression
link |
00:48:35.180
and mating behavior, I think hormones.
link |
00:48:38.440
And whenever there's an opportunity on this podcast
link |
00:48:41.020
to shatter a common myth, I grab it.
link |
00:48:44.700
One of the common myths that's out there
link |
00:48:46.860
and I think that persists is that testosterone
link |
00:48:49.940
makes animals and humans aggressive
link |
00:48:52.780
and estrogen makes animals placid and kind or emotional.
link |
00:48:56.820
And as we both know, nothing could be further
link |
00:48:59.100
from the truth, although there's some truth to the idea
link |
00:49:01.300
that these hormones are all involved.
link |
00:49:03.880
Robert Sapolsky supplied some information to me
link |
00:49:08.340
when he came on this podcast that if you give people
link |
00:49:11.380
exogenous testosterone, it tends to make them more
link |
00:49:13.620
of the way they were before.
link |
00:49:15.500
If they were a jerk before, they'll become more of a jerk.
link |
00:49:17.600
If they were very altruistic, they'll become more altruistic.
link |
00:49:20.100
And then eventually I pointed out you'll aromatize
link |
00:49:22.420
that testosterone and estrogen
link |
00:49:23.820
and you'll start getting opposite effects.
link |
00:49:25.080
So it's a murky space, it's not straightforward.
link |
00:49:28.000
But if I'm not mistaken, testosterone plays a role
link |
00:49:32.140
in generating aggression.
link |
00:49:33.460
However, the specific hormones that are involved
link |
00:49:38.180
in generating aggression via VMH are things other
link |
00:49:43.180
than testosterone.
link |
00:49:44.300
Can you tell us a little bit more about that?
link |
00:49:45.620
Because there's some interesting surprises in there.
link |
00:49:47.740
Yeah, that's a really important question.
link |
00:49:50.220
So when we finally identified the neurons in VMH
link |
00:49:55.420
that control aggression with a molecular marker,
link |
00:49:58.840
we found out that that marker was the estrogen receptor.
link |
00:50:02.660
So that might strike you as a little strange.
link |
00:50:05.100
Why should aggression promoting neurons in male mice
link |
00:50:10.100
be labeled with the estrogen receptor?
link |
00:50:13.540
Other labs have shown that the estrogen receptor
link |
00:50:17.340
in adult male mice is necessary for aggression.
link |
00:50:20.980
If you knock out the gene in VMH, they don't fight.
link |
00:50:24.940
And it's been shown, and a lot of this is work
link |
00:50:27.380
from your colleague Nirav Shah at Stanford,
link |
00:50:29.900
who is one of my former PhD students,
link |
00:50:32.740
that if you castrate a mouse and it loses the ability
link |
00:50:37.740
to fight, not only can you rescue fighting
link |
00:50:41.840
with a testosterone implant, but you can rescue it
link |
00:50:44.960
with an estrogen implant.
link |
00:50:46.780
So you can bypass completely the requirement
link |
00:50:50.040
for testosterone to restore aggressiveness to the mice.
link |
00:50:54.040
And as you say, it's because many of the effects
link |
00:50:57.400
of testosterone, although not all, many of them
link |
00:51:00.840
are mediated by its conversion to estrogen
link |
00:51:04.620
by a process called aromatization.
link |
00:51:07.400
It's carried out by an enzyme called aromatase.
link |
00:51:10.960
In fact, people may have, most of your listeners
link |
00:51:13.580
may have heard of aromatase, because aromatase inhibitors
link |
00:51:17.600
are widely used in female humans
link |
00:51:20.560
as adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer.
link |
00:51:23.600
They are a way of reducing the production of estrogen
link |
00:51:28.840
by preventing testosterone
link |
00:51:31.000
from being converted into estrogen.
link |
00:51:33.240
And in fact, there are a lot of animal experiments
link |
00:51:35.360
showing if you give males aromatase inhibitors,
link |
00:51:38.920
they stop fighting as well as also stop
link |
00:51:42.240
being sexually active.
link |
00:51:44.880
And so that's one of the counterintuitive ideas.
link |
00:51:48.480
And Nirao has shown that progesterone also seems
link |
00:51:52.380
to play a role in aggression,
link |
00:51:54.160
because these aggression neurons also express
link |
00:51:57.060
the progesterone receptor.
link |
00:51:58.660
So here are two hormones that are classically thought of
link |
00:52:02.200
as female reproductive hormones.
link |
00:52:04.680
This is what goes up and goes down during the estrus cycle,
link |
00:52:08.120
estrogen and progesterone,
link |
00:52:10.080
and yet they're playing a very important role
link |
00:52:13.360
in controlling aggression in male mice,
link |
00:52:17.200
and presumably in male humans as well.
link |
00:52:20.000
Fascinating.
link |
00:52:21.280
So estrogen is doing many more things
link |
00:52:24.760
than I think most people believe,
link |
00:52:26.420
and testosterone is doing maybe different
link |
00:52:29.500
and fewer things in some cases and more in others.
link |
00:52:32.120
I've known some aggressive females
link |
00:52:35.920
over the time I've been alive.
link |
00:52:38.480
What's involved in female aggression that's unique
link |
00:52:41.280
from the pathways that generate male aggression?
link |
00:52:43.920
Great, great question.
link |
00:52:45.720
So we and other labs have studied this
link |
00:52:49.160
in both mice and also in fruit flies.
link |
00:52:52.500
So one thing in mice that distinguishes aggression
link |
00:52:56.880
in females from males is that male mice
link |
00:52:59.160
are pretty much ready to fight at the drop of a hat.
link |
00:53:02.240
Female mice only fight when they are nurturing
link |
00:53:08.040
and nursing their pups after they've delivered a litter,
link |
00:53:11.800
and there's a window there
link |
00:53:13.660
where they become hyper aggressive,
link |
00:53:16.600
and then after their pups are weaned,
link |
00:53:19.160
that aggressiveness goes away.
link |
00:53:20.700
So this is pretty remarkable
link |
00:53:23.200
that you take a virgin female mouse
link |
00:53:25.960
and expose it to a male,
link |
00:53:27.600
and her response is to become sexually receptive
link |
00:53:30.640
and to mate with him,
link |
00:53:32.040
and now you let her have her pups
link |
00:53:34.560
and you put the same male or another male mouse
link |
00:53:37.520
in the cage with her,
link |
00:53:38.540
and instead of trying to mate with him, she attacks him.
link |
00:53:41.640
So there is some presumably hormonal
link |
00:53:44.920
and also neuronal switch that's occurring in the brain
link |
00:53:49.080
that switches the response of the female
link |
00:53:52.120
from sex to aggression
link |
00:53:55.040
when she goes from virginity to maternity,
link |
00:53:57.640
and we recently showed in a paper,
link |
00:53:59.740
this is work from one of my students, Mungu Liu,
link |
00:54:03.140
that within VMH in females,
link |
00:54:05.560
there are two clearly divisible subsets
link |
00:54:09.960
of estrogen receptor neurons,
link |
00:54:12.660
and she showed that one of those subsets controls fighting
link |
00:54:17.120
and the other one controls mating,
link |
00:54:19.400
and in fact, if you stimulate
link |
00:54:21.120
the fighting-specific subset in a virgin,
link |
00:54:24.960
you can get the virgin to attack,
link |
00:54:27.160
which is something that we were never able to do before,
link |
00:54:31.240
and if you stimulate the mating one, you enhance mating.
link |
00:54:34.580
The reason we could never get these results
link |
00:54:37.040
when we stimulated the whole population
link |
00:54:39.960
of estrogen receptor neurons
link |
00:54:41.560
is that these effects are opposite and they cancel out,
link |
00:54:45.320
and so it turns out that if you measure the activity
link |
00:54:49.120
of the fighting and the mating neurons
link |
00:54:51.380
going from a virgin to a maternal female,
link |
00:54:55.240
the aggression neurons are very low
link |
00:54:58.360
in their activity in the virgin,
link |
00:55:01.180
but once the female has pups,
link |
00:55:04.160
the activation ability of those neurons goes way up
link |
00:55:08.800
and the mating neurons stay the same,
link |
00:55:11.060
so if you think of the balance between them like a seesaw,
link |
00:55:14.720
in the virgin, there is more activity in the mating neurons
link |
00:55:19.080
than in the fighting neurons,
link |
00:55:20.440
whereas in the nursing mother,
link |
00:55:23.040
there's more activity or more activation
link |
00:55:25.720
in the other way around, the fighting neurons in the mating.
link |
00:55:29.000
Did I say fighting and mating in the first?
link |
00:55:31.240
Mating neurons dominate fighting in the virgin,
link |
00:55:33.920
fighting neurons dominate mating in the mother,
link |
00:55:37.500
so that's a really cool observation,
link |
00:55:41.720
and it's not something that happens in males,
link |
00:55:44.200
and we don't know what causes that or controls that.
link |
00:55:48.440
Interestingly, this gets into the whole issue
link |
00:55:52.640
of neurons that are present in females but not in males,
link |
00:55:57.180
so we've known for, the field has known for a long time
link |
00:56:00.560
that male and female fruit flies have sex-specific neurons,
link |
00:56:04.760
and most of the neurons that we've identified
link |
00:56:07.380
in fruit flies that control fighting in males
link |
00:56:10.440
are male-specific.
link |
00:56:11.760
They're not found in the female brain,
link |
00:56:14.400
but recently, we discovered a set of female-specific
link |
00:56:18.800
fighting neurons in the female brain,
link |
00:56:21.620
together with a couple of other laboratories.
link |
00:56:24.600
Now, they do share one common population of neurons
link |
00:56:28.900
in both male and female flies
link |
00:56:31.240
that in females activates the female-specific
link |
00:56:34.480
fighting neurons and in males activates
link |
00:56:37.160
the male-specific fighting neurons,
link |
00:56:38.900
so it's kind of a hierarchy with this common neuron on top,
link |
00:56:42.720
and in mice, we discovered that there are
link |
00:56:46.560
male-specific neurons in VMH,
link |
00:56:49.760
and those neurons are activated during male aggression.
link |
00:56:54.160
Now, the neurons that are active in females
link |
00:56:57.040
when females fight in VMH are not sex-specific,
link |
00:57:01.020
so they are also found in males,
link |
00:57:03.920
so this is already showing you some complexity.
link |
00:57:06.420
The male mouse VMH has both male-specific
link |
00:57:10.480
aggression neurons and generic aggression neurons,
link |
00:57:13.920
and then the female VMH, the mating cells,
link |
00:57:17.080
are only found in females.
link |
00:57:18.760
They are female-specific and not found in the male brain,
link |
00:57:22.320
and so we're trying to find out
link |
00:57:23.880
what these sex-specific populations of neurons are doing,
link |
00:57:27.320
but that indicates that that is some of the mechanism
link |
00:57:30.760
by which different sexes show different behaviors.
link |
00:57:34.160
I'm fixated on this transition
link |
00:57:36.920
from the virgin female mouse to the maternal female mouse,
link |
00:57:39.760
I have a couple of questions about whether or not,
link |
00:57:42.500
for instance, the transition is governed
link |
00:57:45.260
by the presence of pups.
link |
00:57:46.440
So for instance, if you take a virgin female,
link |
00:57:48.080
she'll mate with a male.
link |
00:57:50.140
Once she's had pups, she'll try and fight that male
link |
00:57:53.360
or presumably another intruder female, right?
link |
00:57:56.280
Equally towards females and male intruders.
link |
00:57:59.040
Does that require the presence of her pups,
link |
00:58:00.980
meaning if you were to take those pups
link |
00:58:02.420
and give them to another mother,
link |
00:58:03.960
does she revert to the more virgin-like behavior?
link |
00:58:06.720
Is it related to, is it triggered by lactation
link |
00:58:09.160
or could it actually be triggered
link |
00:58:10.980
by the mating behavior itself?
link |
00:58:12.660
Because it's possible for the virgin to become a non-virgin,
link |
00:58:16.060
but not actually have a litter of pups.
link |
00:58:18.280
Right, those are all great questions
link |
00:58:20.560
and we don't know the answer to most of them.
link |
00:58:23.480
What I can say is that a nursing mother
link |
00:58:27.360
doesn't have to have her pups with her in the cage
link |
00:58:30.640
in order to attack an intruder male or an intruder female.
link |
00:58:35.420
She is just in a state of brain
link |
00:58:40.180
that makes her aggressive to any intruder.
link |
00:58:43.420
And those aggression neurons in that female's brain
link |
00:58:47.200
are activated by both male and female intruders equally.
link |
00:58:51.860
Whereas in male mice, the aggression neurons
link |
00:58:54.920
are only ever activated by males, not by females,
link |
00:58:59.140
because males are never supposed to attack females.
link |
00:59:02.380
They're only supposed to mate with them.
link |
00:59:04.580
So that's another difference in how those neurons
link |
00:59:07.300
are tuned to signals from different conspecifics.
link |
00:59:11.580
Does it require lactation?
link |
00:59:13.860
I don't know the answer to that.
link |
00:59:15.420
I think there are some experiments
link |
00:59:17.300
where people have tried to, classical experiments,
link |
00:59:20.420
people have tried to reproduce the changes in hormones
link |
00:59:24.540
that occur during pregnancy in female rats
link |
00:59:28.060
to see if it can make them aggressive.
link |
00:59:30.180
And some of those manipulations do to some extent,
link |
00:59:33.820
but there's a whole biology there
link |
00:59:36.140
that remains to be explored
link |
00:59:38.300
about how much of this is hormones,
link |
00:59:40.700
how much of this is circuitry and electricity,
link |
00:59:43.840
and how much of it is other factors
link |
00:59:46.580
that we haven't identified yet.
link |
00:59:48.680
I don't want to anthropomorphize,
link |
00:59:50.060
but well, I'll just ask the question.
link |
00:59:52.900
So the other day I was watching ferrets mate, right?
link |
00:59:56.500
Mustelids, they're mustelids and they're mating behavior.
link |
01:00:00.620
I guess I didn't say why I was watching this.
link |
01:00:02.260
Doesn't matter, it simply doesn't matter.
link |
01:00:05.940
But if one observes the mating behaviors
link |
01:00:08.460
of different animals,
link |
01:00:09.580
we know that there's a tremendous range
link |
01:00:11.320
of mating behaviors in humans.
link |
01:00:13.400
There can be no aggressive component,
link |
01:00:15.500
there could be aggressive component.
link |
01:00:16.580
Humans have all sorts of kinks and fetishes and behaviors,
link |
01:00:19.800
and most of which probably has never been documented
link |
01:00:21.780
because most of this happens in private.
link |
01:00:23.700
And here I always say on this podcast,
link |
01:00:24.980
anytime we're talking about sexual behavior in humans,
link |
01:00:27.540
we're always making the presumption
link |
01:00:28.780
that it's consensual, age appropriate,
link |
01:00:31.320
context appropriate, and species appropriate.
link |
01:00:33.380
Let's say we're talking about a lot of different species.
link |
01:00:35.520
With that said, just to set context,
link |
01:00:38.940
I was watching this video of ferrets mating
link |
01:00:41.900
and it's quite violent actually.
link |
01:00:44.500
There's a lot of neck biting, there's a lot of squealing.
link |
01:00:47.260
If I were going to project an anthropomorphize,
link |
01:00:49.380
I'd say it's not really clear they both want to be there.
link |
01:00:53.740
One would make that assumption.
link |
01:00:54.860
And of course we don't know, we have no idea.
link |
01:00:57.100
This could be the ritual.
link |
01:00:58.620
But it seems to me that there is some crossover
link |
01:01:02.740
of aggression and mating behavior circuitry
link |
01:01:06.460
during the act of mating.
link |
01:01:08.580
And do you think that reflects this sort of
link |
01:01:13.520
stew of competing neurons
link |
01:01:15.780
that are prioritizing in real time?
link |
01:01:19.120
Because of course, as states, they have persistence,
link |
01:01:22.420
as you point out.
link |
01:01:23.260
And you can imagine that states overlapping
link |
01:01:25.220
in four different states, the motivational drive to mate,
link |
01:01:27.740
the motivational drive to get away from this experience,
link |
01:01:30.340
the motivational drive to eat at some point,
link |
01:01:34.660
to defecate at some point,
link |
01:01:35.900
all of these things are competing.
link |
01:01:37.420
And what we're really seeing is a bias in probabilities.
link |
01:01:41.280
But when you look at mating behavior of various animals,
link |
01:01:44.100
you see an aggressive component sometimes, but not always.
link |
01:01:47.760
Is it species specific?
link |
01:01:49.260
Is it context specific?
link |
01:01:51.260
And more generally, do you think that there is crosstalk
link |
01:01:54.900
between these different neuronal populations
link |
01:01:57.100
and the animal itself might be kind of confused
link |
01:01:58.940
about what's going on?
link |
01:01:59.980
Right, great, great questions.
link |
01:02:02.300
I can't really speak to the issue
link |
01:02:04.180
of whether this is species specific
link |
01:02:06.500
because I'm not a naturalist or a zoologist.
link |
01:02:09.240
I've seen, like you have, in the wild,
link |
01:02:11.620
for example, lions when they mate.
link |
01:02:14.020
I've seen them in Africa.
link |
01:02:15.180
There's often a biting component of that as well.
link |
01:02:18.500
One of the things that surprised us
link |
01:02:20.620
when we identified neurons in VMHVL
link |
01:02:24.440
that control aggression in males
link |
01:02:27.260
is that within that population,
link |
01:02:29.940
there is a subset of neurons that is activated by females
link |
01:02:34.820
during male-female mating encounters.
link |
01:02:37.380
Now, you don't generally think of mouse sex as rough sex,
link |
01:02:43.400
but there is a lot of what superficially looks
link |
01:02:48.420
like violent behavior sometimes,
link |
01:02:50.520
especially if the female rejects the male and runs away.
link |
01:02:54.940
And there's some evidence
link |
01:02:57.420
that those female selective neurons in VMH
link |
01:03:02.020
are part of the mating behavior.
link |
01:03:06.460
If you shut them down, the animals don't mate
link |
01:03:10.100
as effectively as they otherwise would.
link |
01:03:13.260
What happens when you stimulate them?
link |
01:03:15.500
We don't yet know because we don't have a way
link |
01:03:17.660
to specifically do that without activating
link |
01:03:20.860
the male aggression neurons.
link |
01:03:22.820
But I think they must be there for a reason
link |
01:03:26.460
because VMH is not traditionally the brain region
link |
01:03:30.420
to which male sexual behavior has been assigned.
link |
01:03:34.140
That's another area called the medial preoptic area.
link |
01:03:38.020
And there we have shown that there are neurons
link |
01:03:41.100
that definitely stimulate mating behavior.
link |
01:03:44.120
In fact, if we activate those mating neurons
link |
01:03:46.980
in a male while it's in the middle
link |
01:03:48.740
of attacking another male, it will stop fighting,
link |
01:03:52.460
start singing to that male and start to try
link |
01:03:55.400
to mount that male until we shut those neurons off.
link |
01:03:58.900
So those are the make love not war neurons
link |
01:04:02.260
and VMH are the make war not love neurons.
link |
01:04:05.460
And there are dense interconnections
link |
01:04:08.140
between these two nuclei,
link |
01:04:10.500
which are very close to each other in the brain.
link |
01:04:13.740
And we've shown that some of those connections
link |
01:04:16.380
are mutually inhibitory to prevent the animal
link |
01:04:20.340
from attacking a mate that it's supposed to be mating with
link |
01:04:24.780
or to prevent it from mating with an animal
link |
01:04:28.500
it's supposed to be attacking.
link |
01:04:29.980
But it's also possible that there are
link |
01:04:32.180
some cooperative interactions between those structures
link |
01:04:36.260
as well as antagonistic interactions
link |
01:04:39.960
and the balance of whether it's the cooperative
link |
01:04:42.900
or antagonistic interactions that are firing
link |
01:04:46.140
at any given moment in a mating encounter,
link |
01:04:49.060
as you suggest, may determine whether a moment
link |
01:04:54.180
of coital bliss among two lions may suddenly turn
link |
01:05:00.460
into a snap or a growl and a bearing of fangs.
link |
01:05:04.480
We don't know that, but certainly the substrate,
link |
01:05:07.840
the wiring is there for that to happen.
link |
01:05:10.980
I'm sure people's minds are running wild with all this.
link |
01:05:14.100
I'll just use this as an opportunity to raise something
link |
01:05:16.780
I've wondered about for far too long,
link |
01:05:19.840
which is I have a friend who's a psychiatrist
link |
01:05:23.180
who works on the treatment of fetishes.
link |
01:05:25.540
This is not a psychiatrist that I was treated by.
link |
01:05:27.660
I'll just point that out.
link |
01:05:29.260
But they mentioned something very interesting to me
link |
01:05:32.100
long ago, which is that when you look at true fetishes
link |
01:05:36.140
and what meets the criteria for fetish,
link |
01:05:38.180
that there does seem to be some,
link |
01:05:40.820
what one would think would be competing circuitry
link |
01:05:43.780
that suddenly becomes aligned.
link |
01:05:46.940
For instance, avoidance of feces, dead bodies, feet,
link |
01:05:52.820
things that are very infectious.
link |
01:05:55.120
Typically, those states of disgust are antagonistic
link |
01:05:58.820
to states of desire, as one would hope is present
link |
01:06:04.200
during sexual behavior.
link |
01:06:05.720
Fetishes often involve exactly those things
link |
01:06:09.660
that are aversive, feet, dead bodies,
link |
01:06:15.060
disgusting things to most people.
link |
01:06:17.500
And true fetishes in the pathologic sense
link |
01:06:21.220
exist when people have basically a requirement
link |
01:06:24.840
for thinking about, or even the presence
link |
01:06:26.500
of those ordinarily disgusting things
link |
01:06:29.500
in order to become sexually aroused,
link |
01:06:31.420
as if the circuitry has crossed over.
link |
01:06:33.760
And the statement that rung in my mind was
link |
01:06:35.940
people don't develop fetishes to mailboxes
link |
01:06:39.500
or to the color red or to random objects and things.
link |
01:06:44.220
They develop fetishes to things that are highly infectious
link |
01:06:47.220
and counter reproductive, repetitive states.
link |
01:06:50.140
So I find that interesting.
link |
01:06:51.900
I don't know if you have any reflections on that
link |
01:06:53.820
as to why that might be.
link |
01:06:55.660
I'm tempted to ask whether or not you've ever observed
link |
01:06:57.980
fetish-like behavior in mice,
link |
01:06:59.420
but I find it fascinating that you have this area
link |
01:07:02.460
of the brain that's so highly conserved, the hypothalamus,
link |
01:07:04.840
which you have these dense populations intermixed
link |
01:07:06.980
and that the addition of a forebrain,
link |
01:07:09.820
especially in humans that can think and make decisions
link |
01:07:12.860
could in some ways facilitate the expression
link |
01:07:15.860
of these primitive behaviors,
link |
01:07:16.960
but could also complicate the expression
link |
01:07:19.140
of primitive behaviors.
link |
01:07:20.540
Right, I would agree.
link |
01:07:22.700
I think one way of looking at fetishes
link |
01:07:26.100
from a neurobiological standpoint
link |
01:07:28.660
is that they represent a kind of repetitive conditioning
link |
01:07:33.660
where something that is natively aversive or disgusting
link |
01:07:39.440
by being repeatedly paired with a rewarding experience
link |
01:07:44.880
changes its valence, its sign,
link |
01:07:49.140
so that now it somehow produces the anticipation of reward
link |
01:07:55.320
the next time a person sees it.
link |
01:07:57.840
Now, I don't know how,
link |
01:07:58.720
I don't know that literature in animals,
link |
01:08:01.160
so I don't know if you could condition a mouse
link |
01:08:04.760
to eat feces, for example,
link |
01:08:06.480
although there are animals that are naturally coprophagic.
link |
01:08:10.320
That is, and maybe mice do that occasionally, I'm not sure,
link |
01:08:14.680
but that is one way to think about it,
link |
01:08:18.080
and that could certainly involve in humans
link |
01:08:20.880
the more recently evolved parts of the brain,
link |
01:08:23.360
the cortex that is sort of orchestrating
link |
01:08:27.360
both what behaviors are happening
link |
01:08:29.920
and whether reward states are turning on
link |
01:08:33.320
in association with those behaviors that are happening,
link |
01:08:37.040
and that's the part that I think is difficult
link |
01:08:41.240
and challenging to study in a mouse,
link |
01:08:44.340
but certainly bears thinking about
link |
01:08:48.280
because it's a really interesting,
link |
01:08:50.820
again, sort of counterintuitive aspect,
link |
01:08:53.760
again, like rough sex,
link |
01:08:55.000
people that want to have fighting or violence
link |
01:08:58.040
or aggressiveness in order to be sexually aroused
link |
01:09:01.080
and fetishes, and in fact,
link |
01:09:03.760
when we made that discovery initially,
link |
01:09:05.800
it raised the question in my mind
link |
01:09:08.060
whether some people that are serial rapists,
link |
01:09:13.720
for example, and engage in sexual violence
link |
01:09:16.800
might in some level have their wires crossed in some way
link |
01:09:20.920
that these states that are supposed to be
link |
01:09:23.680
pretty much separated and mutually antagonistic
link |
01:09:26.880
are not and are actually more rewarding and reinforcing.
link |
01:09:31.480
I think it's gonna be a long time
link |
01:09:33.880
before we have figured it out,
link |
01:09:36.000
but when you think about it,
link |
01:09:37.800
there is no treatment that we have
link |
01:09:41.440
for a violent sexual offender that eliminates the violence,
link |
01:09:46.600
but not the sexual desire and sexual urge,
link |
01:09:50.600
whether it's physical castration or chemical castration,
link |
01:09:54.820
it eliminates both.
link |
01:09:57.280
Definitely an area that I think,
link |
01:09:59.280
well, human neuroscience in general needs a lot of tools,
link |
01:10:03.440
in terms of how to probe and manipulate neural circuitry.
link |
01:10:06.800
I'd love to turn to this area that you mentioned,
link |
01:10:09.740
the medial preoptic area.
link |
01:10:12.020
I'm fascinated by it because just as within the VMH,
link |
01:10:15.040
you have these neurons for mating and fighting or aggression,
link |
01:10:19.140
my understanding is medial preoptic area
link |
01:10:21.660
contains neurons for mating, but also for
link |
01:10:24.840
temperature regulation.
link |
01:10:26.360
And perhaps I'm making too much of a leap here,
link |
01:10:28.480
but I've always wondered about this phrase in heat.
link |
01:10:32.040
As certainly the menstrual or estrous cycle in females
link |
01:10:37.000
is related to changes in body temperature.
link |
01:10:38.880
In fact, measuring body temperature is one way
link |
01:10:40.560
that women can fairly reliably predict ovulation, et cetera.
link |
01:10:45.720
Although additional, this is not a show about contraception,
link |
01:10:48.380
please rely on multiple methods as necessary.
link |
01:10:50.560
Don't use this discussion as your guide for contraception
link |
01:10:54.320
based on temperature.
link |
01:10:55.360
But if you stimulate certain neurons
link |
01:10:58.200
in the medial preoptic area,
link |
01:10:59.360
you can trigger dramatic changes in body temperature
link |
01:11:03.480
and or mating behavior.
link |
01:11:06.160
What's the relationship, if any,
link |
01:11:07.480
between temperature and mating, or do we simply not know?
link |
01:11:11.640
I don't know what the relationship is
link |
01:11:15.160
between temperature and mating neurons
link |
01:11:18.800
in the preoptic area.
link |
01:11:20.960
I suspect that they are different populations of neurons
link |
01:11:25.360
because it's become pretty clear that the preoptic area
link |
01:11:29.840
has many different subsets of neurons
link |
01:11:32.560
that are specifically active during different behaviors,
link |
01:11:36.200
even different phases of mating behavior.
link |
01:11:38.780
So there are mounting neurons,
link |
01:11:40.560
there are intermission, thrusting neurons,
link |
01:11:43.160
and ejaculation neurons, and sniffing neurons.
link |
01:11:46.080
Wait, wait, so I think I've heard this before,
link |
01:11:48.640
but I just wanna make sure that people get this,
link |
01:11:50.240
and I wanna make sure I get this.
link |
01:11:51.440
So you're telling me within the medial preoptic area,
link |
01:11:55.540
there are specific neurons that if you stimulate them
link |
01:11:57.840
will make males thrust as if they're mating?
link |
01:12:00.800
No, so this is not based on stimulation experiments.
link |
01:12:05.960
It's based on imaging experiments right now
link |
01:12:08.940
that we see when we look in the preoptic area
link |
01:12:11.840
at what neurons are active
link |
01:12:14.140
during different phases of aggression,
link |
01:12:17.280
we see that there are different neurons
link |
01:12:19.280
that are active during sniffing,
link |
01:12:21.480
mounting, thrusting, and ejaculation,
link |
01:12:24.920
and they become repeatedly activated
link |
01:12:28.040
each time the animal goes through that cycle.
link |
01:12:30.280
During mating.
link |
01:12:31.120
During the mating cycle.
link |
01:12:32.920
There are also some neurons there
link |
01:12:34.400
that are active during aggression, which are distinct,
link |
01:12:37.400
and we don't know whether those neurons are there
link |
01:12:40.480
to promote aggression or to inhibit mating
link |
01:12:44.660
when animals are fighting.
link |
01:12:46.240
We have some evidence that suggests it may be the latter,
link |
01:12:49.560
but we don't know for sure yet.
link |
01:12:51.440
The thermosensitive neurons are really interesting
link |
01:12:54.240
because you mentioned the phrase in heat,
link |
01:12:57.480
and then in the context of aggression,
link |
01:12:59.820
you talk about hot-blooded people or hotheads.
link |
01:13:02.960
There's just recently a paper showing
link |
01:13:04.760
there are thermoregulatory neurons in VMH as well.
link |
01:13:08.900
So all of these homeostatic systems for metabolic control
link |
01:13:13.560
and temperature control are intermingled in these nuclei,
link |
01:13:18.600
these zones that control these basic survival behaviors
link |
01:13:22.900
like mating and aggression and predator defense.
link |
01:13:26.900
And I would imagine that the thermoregulation
link |
01:13:31.280
is tightly connected to energy expenditure
link |
01:13:35.560
and that, again, these neurons are mixed together
link |
01:13:39.180
to facilitate integration of all these signals
link |
01:13:43.080
by the brain in some way that we don't understand
link |
01:13:46.640
to maintain the proper balance
link |
01:13:48.520
between energy conservation and energy consumption
link |
01:13:53.640
during this particular behavior or that behavior.
link |
01:13:56.800
I mean, I've always been fascinated by the question,
link |
01:13:58.980
why is it that violence goes up in the summertime
link |
01:14:03.040
when the temperatures are high?
link |
01:14:04.840
Does it really have something to do with the idea
link |
01:14:08.400
that increased temperature increases violence?
link |
01:14:12.080
Seems hard to believe because we're homeothermic
link |
01:14:15.360
and we pretty much stay around 98.6 Fahrenheit.
link |
01:14:20.720
Could be other social reasons why that happens.
link |
01:14:23.640
People are outside out on the street
link |
01:14:25.600
bumping into each other.
link |
01:14:27.080
But I think there could well be something
link |
01:14:30.280
that ties thermoregulation to aggressiveness
link |
01:14:34.040
as well as to mating behavior.
link |
01:14:38.200
Fascinating, Gal.
link |
01:14:39.160
I asked in the hopes that maybe in the years to come,
link |
01:14:43.360
your lab will parse some of the temperature relationships.
link |
01:14:46.400
And I realized it could be also regulated
link |
01:14:49.320
by hormones in general.
link |
01:14:50.480
So it's tapping into two systems
link |
01:14:51.880
for completely different reasons.
link |
01:14:53.080
But anyway, an area that intrigues me
link |
01:14:57.000
because of this notion of hotheadedness
link |
01:14:58.600
or cool, common, collected.
link |
01:15:00.440
And also the fact that,
link |
01:15:02.880
I probably should have asked about this earlier,
link |
01:15:04.400
that arousal itself is tethered
link |
01:15:07.240
to the whole mating and reproductive process.
link |
01:15:09.640
I mean, without a sort of seesawing back
link |
01:15:12.040
between the sympathetic and parasympathetic arousal,
link |
01:15:16.080
relaxed states, there is no mating that will take place.
link |
01:15:20.760
So it's fascinating the way these different competing forces
link |
01:15:23.440
and seesaws operate.
link |
01:15:25.760
Several times during the discussion so far,
link |
01:15:27.920
we've hit on this idea that the same behavior
link |
01:15:32.940
can reflect different states
link |
01:15:34.760
and different states can converge on multiple behaviors
link |
01:15:38.400
as well.
link |
01:15:39.380
You had a paper not long ago about mounting behavior,
link |
01:15:43.320
which I found fascinating.
link |
01:15:45.520
Maybe you could tell us about that result
link |
01:15:47.760
because to me, it really speaks to the fact
link |
01:15:49.800
that mounting behavior can in one context be sexual
link |
01:15:53.880
and in another context actually be related
link |
01:15:55.840
to we presume dominance.
link |
01:15:57.960
And I think that my friends who practice jujitsu
link |
01:16:02.640
will say they, when I talk about that result,
link |
01:16:04.840
they say, of course, you know,
link |
01:16:05.880
mounting the other person and dominating them,
link |
01:16:08.800
there's nothing sexual about it.
link |
01:16:10.200
It's about overtaking them physically,
link |
01:16:12.380
literally being on their next side
link |
01:16:14.320
as opposed to on their own, lying on their own back.
link |
01:16:18.360
Just fascinating, very primitive.
link |
01:16:20.060
And yet I think speaks to this idea
link |
01:16:23.060
that mounting behavior might be one
link |
01:16:24.880
of the most fundamental ways in which animals
link |
01:16:27.680
and perhaps even humans express dominance
link |
01:16:30.840
and or sexual interactions.
link |
01:16:34.020
Yep, that's a fascinating question.
link |
01:16:37.500
And it was harder to figure out
link |
01:16:39.840
than you might've thought.
link |
01:16:41.200
So we've, there's been this debate for a long time
link |
01:16:44.160
in the field when you see two male mice mounting each other,
link |
01:16:48.700
is this homosexual behavior?
link |
01:16:50.860
Is this a case of mistaken sexual identification
link |
01:16:54.280
or is this dominance behavior?
link |
01:16:56.400
And if you train an AI algorithm
link |
01:16:59.520
to try to distinguish male-male mounting
link |
01:17:03.800
from male-female mounting, it does not do a very good job
link |
01:17:07.120
because motorically those behaviors look so similar.
link |
01:17:11.440
And so how did we wind up figuring out
link |
01:17:16.320
that most male-male mounting is dominance mounting?
link |
01:17:20.900
There are two important clues.
link |
01:17:24.040
One is the context.
link |
01:17:26.880
And so male-male mounting tends
link |
01:17:29.640
to be more prominent among mice
link |
01:17:32.360
when they haven't had a lot of fighting experience.
link |
01:17:35.600
And then as they become more experienced in fighting,
link |
01:17:40.420
they will show relatively less mounting
link |
01:17:43.720
towards the other male and more attack.
link |
01:17:46.080
And they'll transition quickly from mounting to attack.
link |
01:17:50.140
And so the mounting is always seen in this context
link |
01:17:54.560
of an overall aggressive interaction.
link |
01:17:58.000
And then the second thing, which believe it or not,
link |
01:18:00.240
was suggested by a computational theoretical person
link |
01:18:04.780
in my lab, Anne Kennedy,
link |
01:18:06.840
who now has her own lab at Northwestern.
link |
01:18:09.160
She said, well, males are known to sing
link |
01:18:12.500
when they mount females, ultrasonic vocalizations.
link |
01:18:15.600
Why don't you see what kinds of songs they're singing
link |
01:18:18.960
when they're mounting males?
link |
01:18:20.280
Maybe it's a different kind of song.
link |
01:18:21.800
Well, what we found out is they don't sing at all
link |
01:18:25.120
when they're mounting a male.
link |
01:18:26.880
So you can easily distinguish whether mounting behavior
link |
01:18:31.880
by a male mouse is reproductive or agonistic,
link |
01:18:36.920
aggressive according to whether it's accompanied
link |
01:18:40.200
by ultrasonic vocalizations or not.
link |
01:18:43.580
And it turns out that different brain regions
link |
01:18:47.080
are maximally active
link |
01:18:49.120
during these different types of mounting.
link |
01:18:51.080
So VMH, the aggression locus,
link |
01:18:55.120
is actually active during dominance mounting,
link |
01:18:58.380
and you can stimulate mounting if you,
link |
01:19:01.560
dominance mounting, if you weakly activate VMH,
link |
01:19:05.440
whereas MPOA is most strongly activated
link |
01:19:08.880
during sexual mounting,
link |
01:19:10.820
and that's always accompanied
link |
01:19:12.800
by the ultrasonic vocalization.
link |
01:19:14.720
So this shows how difficult and dangerous it can be
link |
01:19:18.520
to try to infer an animal's state or intent or emotion
link |
01:19:23.360
from the behavior that it's exhibiting,
link |
01:19:25.840
because the same behavior can mean very different things
link |
01:19:29.100
depending on the context
link |
01:19:30.480
or the interaction with the animal.
link |
01:19:32.160
And I would say even more so
link |
01:19:33.820
with when that animal is a human or is multiple humans.
link |
01:19:37.640
That's right.
link |
01:19:38.480
And there are many examples.
link |
01:19:40.400
Animals show chasing to obtain food,
link |
01:19:44.400
a prey animal that they're gonna kill and eat,
link |
01:19:47.060
and they show chasing to obtain a mate
link |
01:19:49.840
that they're gonna have sex with.
link |
01:19:51.620
And so the intent of the chasing is completely different,
link |
01:19:55.560
and we don't know in all these cases
link |
01:19:57.400
whether there are separate circuits
link |
01:19:59.520
or common circuits that are being activated.
link |
01:20:02.700
I'm obsessed with dogs and dog breeds
link |
01:20:04.800
and et cetera, et cetera.
link |
01:20:06.140
And one thing I can tell you
link |
01:20:08.760
is that female dogs will mount and thrust.
link |
01:20:12.760
We had a female pit bull mix, very sweet dog,
link |
01:20:17.040
but in observing her, it convinced me
link |
01:20:20.640
that one can never assume
link |
01:20:22.880
that male dogs are more aggressive than female dogs.
link |
01:20:25.680
There's a, it turns out in talking to people
link |
01:20:27.760
who are quite skilled at dog genetics and dog breeding,
link |
01:20:31.280
that there's a dominance hierarchy within a litter,
link |
01:20:33.920
and it crosses over male-female delineations.
link |
01:20:39.480
So you can get a female in the litter
link |
01:20:40.840
that's very dominant, a male that's very subordinate,
link |
01:20:43.200
and no one really knows what relates to.
link |
01:20:45.880
This is also why little dogs sometimes
link |
01:20:47.440
will get right up in the face of a big Doberman Pinscher
link |
01:20:50.600
and just start barking,
link |
01:20:51.440
which is an idiotic thing for it to do,
link |
01:20:53.080
but they can be dominant over a much larger dog.
link |
01:20:56.280
Very strange, to me anyway.
link |
01:20:59.480
Female-female mounting, do you observe it in mice?
link |
01:21:03.140
Are there known circuits?
link |
01:21:04.640
And what evokes female-female mounting,
link |
01:21:07.240
or female-to-male mounting, if it occurs?
link |
01:21:10.440
Good.
link |
01:21:11.280
Yes, there is female,
link |
01:21:13.520
there are clear examples of females
link |
01:21:16.460
displaying male-type mounting behavior
link |
01:21:19.080
towards other females.
link |
01:21:20.580
We see this most commonly in the lab
link |
01:21:23.960
where we are housing females with their sister,
link |
01:21:27.360
say three or four in a cage.
link |
01:21:29.580
We take one out and we have her mate with a male
link |
01:21:33.480
where the male's doing the mounting.
link |
01:21:35.260
Now we take that female and we put her back in the cage
link |
01:21:39.040
with her litter mates and she starts mounting them.
link |
01:21:42.840
Now what the function of that is,
link |
01:21:45.960
if it has any function or what it means,
link |
01:21:49.040
what's driving it, we don't know.
link |
01:21:51.160
But we do know that if we stimulate the mount,
link |
01:21:55.480
the neurons that control mounting in males
link |
01:21:58.280
in the medial preoptic area,
link |
01:21:59.800
if we stimulate that same population in females,
link |
01:22:03.760
it evokes male-type mounting
link |
01:22:07.200
towards either a male or a female target.
link |
01:22:09.700
In fact, we have a movie where we have a female
link |
01:22:13.960
that has just been mounted by a male.
link |
01:22:16.560
So the male's on top and she's underneath
link |
01:22:19.640
and we stimulate that region of MPOA in the female
link |
01:22:24.160
and she crawls out from underneath the male
link |
01:22:27.440
who has just mounted her, circles around behind him
link |
01:22:31.560
and climbs up on top of him
link |
01:22:33.400
and starts to try to mount him and thrust at him.
link |
01:22:36.560
That has a name online, it's called a switch.
link |
01:22:39.020
Is that right?
link |
01:22:40.680
Don't ask me how I know that.
link |
01:22:41.760
Okay.
link |
01:22:42.840
But it's a pretty, yeah, it's a term that you hear.
link |
01:22:48.080
You also hear the term topping from the bottom,
link |
01:22:50.640
which it sounds like that is a literal topping
link |
01:22:52.440
from the bottom. I see.
link |
01:22:53.280
That's more of a psychological phrase from what I hear.
link |
01:22:55.760
I have friends that are educating me in this language,
link |
01:22:59.920
mostly because I find this kind
link |
01:23:02.000
of neurobiological discussion fascinating.
link |
01:23:05.160
At some point, right, I attempt in my mind
link |
01:23:08.100
to superimpose observations from the online communities
link |
01:23:11.880
that I've been told about and asked about to this,
link |
01:23:14.440
but I should point out it's always dangerous
link |
01:23:16.520
and in fact, inappropriate to make a one-to-one link.
link |
01:23:20.720
Humans are, they maintain all the same neural circuitry
link |
01:23:23.600
and pathways that we're talking about today in mice,
link |
01:23:26.000
but that forebrain does allow for context, et cetera.
link |
01:23:30.200
Yep.
link |
01:23:31.040
So what the function is of female mounting,
link |
01:23:36.040
I don't know, it could be a type of dominance display.
link |
01:23:39.580
It's hard to measure that because people haven't worked
link |
01:23:42.420
on female dominance hierarchies to the same extent
link |
01:23:46.120
that they've worked on male dominance hierarchies,
link |
01:23:49.100
but it indicates that the circuits for male type mounting
link |
01:23:54.120
are there in females as early work
link |
01:23:56.920
from Catherine Dulock suggested some years ago.
link |
01:24:00.240
Fascinating, fascinating.
link |
01:24:01.760
I love that paper because as you pointed out for Chase,
link |
01:24:04.820
you know, for mounting behavior, you know,
link |
01:24:08.120
we see it and we think one thing specifically.
link |
01:24:11.440
And after hearing this result, actually,
link |
01:24:13.340
I'm not a big fan of fight sports.
link |
01:24:14.960
I watch them occasionally because friends are into them,
link |
01:24:16.880
but I've seen boxing matches, MMA matches,
link |
01:24:21.320
where at the end of a round,
link |
01:24:23.140
if someone felt that they dominated,
link |
01:24:25.120
they will do the unsportsmanlike thing of the thrusting
link |
01:24:29.240
on the back of the other person before they get off,
link |
01:24:30.880
almost like I dominated you.
link |
01:24:32.480
And so mimicking sexual like behavior,
link |
01:24:35.080
but there's no reason to think that it's sexual,
link |
01:24:37.360
but they're sending a message of dominance is what implies.
link |
01:24:41.620
I'd love to talk about something slightly off
link |
01:24:45.520
from this circuitry, but I think that's related
link |
01:24:47.620
to the circuitry, at least in some way,
link |
01:24:49.300
which is this structure that I've always been fascinated by
link |
01:24:52.560
and I can't figure out what the hell it's for
link |
01:24:54.940
because it seems to be involved in everything,
link |
01:24:56.080
which is the PAG, the periaqueductal gray,
link |
01:25:00.320
which is a little bit further back in the brain
link |
01:25:01.800
for people that don't know.
link |
01:25:03.380
It's been studied in the context of pain.
link |
01:25:05.040
It's been studied in the context
link |
01:25:06.600
of the so-called lordosis response,
link |
01:25:08.520
the receptivity or arching of the back of the female
link |
01:25:10.960
to receive intromission and mating from the male.
link |
01:25:14.560
How should we think about PAG?
link |
01:25:17.400
Clearly it can't be involved in everything.
link |
01:25:19.600
I'm guessing it's at least as complex
link |
01:25:21.640
as some of these other regions
link |
01:25:22.640
that we've been talking about,
link |
01:25:23.580
different types of neurons controlling different things,
link |
01:25:25.420
but how does PAG play into this?
link |
01:25:27.880
In particular, I wanna know,
link |
01:25:29.960
is there some mechanism of pain modulation and control
link |
01:25:33.520
during fighting and or mating?
link |
01:25:37.400
And the reason I ask is that,
link |
01:25:39.600
while I'm not a combat sports person,
link |
01:25:42.240
years ago I did a little bit of martial arts
link |
01:25:44.560
and it always was impressive to me
link |
01:25:46.800
how little it hurt to get punched during a fight
link |
01:25:49.320
and how much it hurt afterwards, right?
link |
01:25:51.920
So there clearly is some endogenous pain control
link |
01:25:55.480
that then wears off and then you feel beat up,
link |
01:25:58.760
at least in my case, I felt beat up.
link |
01:26:00.720
What's PAG doing vis-a-vis pain and vis-a-vis,
link |
01:26:04.200
and what's pain doing vis-a-vis these other behaviors?
link |
01:26:06.200
Good, good.
link |
01:26:07.040
So I think of PAG like a old fashioned telephone switchboard
link |
01:26:13.960
where there are calls coming in
link |
01:26:17.460
and then the cables have to be punched into the right hole
link |
01:26:20.840
to get the information to be routed to the right recipient
link |
01:26:24.800
on the other end of it,
link |
01:26:26.480
because pretty much every type of innate behavior
link |
01:26:30.600
you can think of has had the PAG implicated.
link |
01:26:34.360
There's a whole literature showing the involvement
link |
01:26:37.480
of the PAG in fear, different regions of the PAG.
link |
01:26:41.660
The dorsal PAG is involved in panic-like behavior,
link |
01:26:45.320
running away.
link |
01:26:46.360
The ventral PAG is involved in freezing behavior.
link |
01:26:50.900
Both the MPOA and VMH send projections to the PAG,
link |
01:26:55.900
to different regions of the PAG.
link |
01:26:58.900
So in cross-section, I hate to say this,
link |
01:27:02.800
but in cross-section,
link |
01:27:04.200
the PAG kind of looks like the water in a toilet
link |
01:27:06.940
when you're standing over an open toilet bowl.
link |
01:27:09.980
And if you imagine a clock face projected onto that,
link |
01:27:15.040
it's like the PAG has sectors from one to 12,
link |
01:27:19.960
maybe even more of them.
link |
01:27:21.300
And in each of those sectors,
link |
01:27:22.920
you find different neurons from the hypothalamus
link |
01:27:25.740
are projecting.
link |
01:27:26.880
So could turn out that there is a topographic arrangement
link |
01:27:31.220
along the dorsal ventral axis of the PAG
link |
01:27:34.220
and the medial lateral axis of the PAG
link |
01:27:37.020
that determines the type of behavior
link |
01:27:39.860
that will be emitted when neurons in that region
link |
01:27:43.140
are stimulated.
link |
01:27:43.980
And I think sort of all of the evidence
link |
01:27:46.060
is pointing in that direction,
link |
01:27:47.840
but by no means has it been mapped out.
link |
01:27:50.580
Now, the thing that you mentioned about it not hurting
link |
01:27:54.040
when you got beat up during martial arts,
link |
01:27:56.940
there is a well-known phenomenon
link |
01:27:59.260
called fear-induced analgesia,
link |
01:28:03.820
where when an animal is in a high state of fear,
link |
01:28:08.740
like if it's trying to defend itself,
link |
01:28:11.220
there is a suppression of pain responses.
link |
01:28:16.220
And I'm not sure completely about the mechanisms
link |
01:28:20.660
and how well that's understood.
link |
01:28:22.740
But for example, the adrenal gland has a peptide in it
link |
01:28:28.780
that is released from the adrenal medulla,
link |
01:28:32.220
which controls the fight or flight responses.
link |
01:28:34.900
And that peptide has analgesic activities.
link |
01:28:38.940
Now whether- Ask what that peptide is.
link |
01:28:40.020
It's called bovine adrenal medullary peptide
link |
01:28:43.300
of 22 amino acid residues.
link |
01:28:45.480
And I only know about it because it activates a receptor
link |
01:28:49.500
that we discovered many years ago
link |
01:28:51.560
that's involved in pain, and we thought it promoted pain,
link |
01:28:54.780
but it turns out that this actually inhibits pain.
link |
01:28:57.700
It's like an endogenous analgesic.
link |
01:29:00.460
Whether this is happening, this type of analgesia
link |
01:29:05.380
is happening when an animal is engaged
link |
01:29:08.100
in offensive aggression or in mating behavior,
link |
01:29:13.220
I don't know, but it certainly is possible.
link |
01:29:16.420
And I don't know whether these analgesic mechanisms
link |
01:29:20.480
are happening in the PAG.
link |
01:29:22.500
They could also be happening a little further down
link |
01:29:25.540
in the spinal cord.
link |
01:29:26.620
The PAG is really continuous with the spinal cord.
link |
01:29:30.000
If you just follow it down towards the tail of an animal,
link |
01:29:33.900
you will wind up in the spinal cord.
link |
01:29:36.700
And so it could be that there are influences
link |
01:29:40.620
acting at many levels on pain in the PAG
link |
01:29:44.020
and in the spinal cord as well.
link |
01:29:46.380
And it may well be known, I just don't know it.
link |
01:29:49.100
I wanna distinguish clearly between things
link |
01:29:51.780
that are not known, that I know are unknown,
link |
01:29:55.100
which is in a fairly small area where I have expertise
link |
01:29:58.780
from things that may be known, but I'm ignorant of them
link |
01:30:01.820
because I just don't have a broad enough knowledge base
link |
01:30:04.300
to know that.
link |
01:30:05.140
Sure, we appreciate those delineations.
link |
01:30:09.260
Thank you, PAG.
link |
01:30:10.700
I think this description of it
link |
01:30:11.740
is an old-fashioned telephone switchboard.
link |
01:30:14.100
And now every time I look into the toilet,
link |
01:30:16.420
I'll think about the periaqueductal gray.
link |
01:30:18.140
And every time I see an image of periaqueductal gray,
link |
01:30:20.260
I'll think about a toilet.
link |
01:30:21.260
That is an excellent description
link |
01:30:23.660
because in fact, I drew a circle
link |
01:30:25.220
with a little thing at the bottom.
link |
01:30:27.640
Well, I'll put a link to a picture of PAG
link |
01:30:30.300
and you'll understand why David and I are chuckling here
link |
01:30:33.220
because indeed it looks like a toilet
link |
01:30:35.420
when staring into a toilet.
link |
01:30:38.320
Tell us about tachykinin.
link |
01:30:40.360
I've talked about this a couple of times
link |
01:30:41.700
on different podcast episodes
link |
01:30:42.940
because of its relationship to social isolation.
link |
01:30:47.420
And in part because the podcast was launched
link |
01:30:51.100
during a time when there was more social isolation.
link |
01:30:54.660
My understanding is that tachykinin,
link |
01:30:56.820
and you'll tell us what it is in a moment,
link |
01:30:59.360
is present in flies and mice and in humans
link |
01:31:01.820
and may do similar things in those species.
link |
01:31:05.860
That's right.
link |
01:31:06.700
So, tachykinin is,
link |
01:31:09.580
refers to a family of related neuropeptides.
link |
01:31:12.980
So these are brain chemicals.
link |
01:31:15.180
They're different from dopamine and serotonin
link |
01:31:19.420
in that they're not small organic molecules.
link |
01:31:23.080
They're actually short pieces of protein
link |
01:31:25.860
that are directly encoded by genes
link |
01:31:28.760
that are active in specific neurons and not in others.
link |
01:31:32.300
And when those neurons are active,
link |
01:31:34.100
those neuropeptides are released together
link |
01:31:36.900
with classical transmitters like glutamate, whatever.
link |
01:31:40.560
Tachykinins have been famously implicated in pain,
link |
01:31:44.980
particularly tachykinin-1, which is called substance P,
link |
01:31:50.400
one of the original pain modulating.
link |
01:31:53.340
This is something that promotes inflammatory pain.
link |
01:31:57.460
But there are other tachykinin genes.
link |
01:32:00.020
In mice, there are two.
link |
01:32:01.660
In humans, I think there are three.
link |
01:32:03.980
And in Drosophila, there is one.
link |
01:32:06.700
And the way we got into tachykinins
link |
01:32:09.260
is from studying aggression in flies.
link |
01:32:12.540
We thought, since neuropeptides have this remarkable
link |
01:32:17.300
parallel evolutionary conservation of structure
link |
01:32:21.260
and function, like neuropeptide Y controls feeding
link |
01:32:25.460
in worms, in flies, and mice, and in people,
link |
01:32:28.980
oxytocin-like peptides control reproduction
link |
01:32:32.200
in worms and mice and in people,
link |
01:32:35.580
we thought we might find peptides
link |
01:32:37.660
that control aggression in flies and in people.
link |
01:32:40.660
And so we did a screen, unbiased screen of peptides
link |
01:32:44.720
and found indeed that one of the tachykinins,
link |
01:32:48.660
Drosophila tachykinin, those neurons,
link |
01:32:51.580
when you activate them, strongly promote aggression
link |
01:32:54.780
and it depends on the release of tachykinin.
link |
01:32:57.560
Now, the interesting thing is that in flies,
link |
01:33:00.580
just like in people and practically any other social animal
link |
01:33:05.440
that shows aggression,
link |
01:33:06.940
social isolation increases aggressiveness.
link |
01:33:10.420
So putting a violent prisoner in solitary confinement
link |
01:33:14.940
is absolutely the worst,
link |
01:33:16.380
most counterproductive thing you could do to them.
link |
01:33:19.060
And indeed we found in flies that social isolation
link |
01:33:23.060
increases the level of tachykinin in the brain.
link |
01:33:26.900
And if we shut that gene down,
link |
01:33:29.160
it prevents the isolation from increasing aggression.
link |
01:33:32.640
So since my lab also works on mice,
link |
01:33:35.660
it was natural to see whether tachykinins
link |
01:33:38.960
might be upregulated in social isolation
link |
01:33:42.260
and whether they play a role in aggression.
link |
01:33:44.500
And this is work done by a former postdoc,
link |
01:33:46.860
Moriel Zelikovsky,
link |
01:33:48.060
now at University of Salt Lake City in Utah.
link |
01:33:50.860
And she found remarkably that when mice
link |
01:33:54.720
are socially isolated for two weeks,
link |
01:33:56.880
there is this massive upregulation of tachykinin II
link |
01:34:02.460
in their brain.
link |
01:34:03.420
In fact, if you tag the peptide
link |
01:34:06.300
with a green fluorescent protein from a jellyfish,
link |
01:34:09.940
genetically, the brain looks green
link |
01:34:12.940
when the mice are socially isolated
link |
01:34:14.940
because there's so much of this stuff released.
link |
01:34:17.740
And she went on to show that that increase in tachykinin
link |
01:34:24.100
is responsible for the effect of social isolation
link |
01:34:27.820
to increase aggressiveness and to increase fear
link |
01:34:31.620
and to increase anxiety.
link |
01:34:33.260
And in fact, there are drugs that block the receptor
link |
01:34:36.860
for tachykinin, which were tested in humans and abandoned
link |
01:34:41.180
because they had no efficacy in the tests
link |
01:34:43.820
that they were analyzed for.
link |
01:34:45.600
If you give those drugs to a socially isolated mouse,
link |
01:34:49.760
it blocks all of the effects of social isolation.
link |
01:34:53.420
It blocks the aggression,
link |
01:34:55.340
it blocks the increased fear and the increased anxiety.
link |
01:34:58.920
And that Moriel described it, the mice just look chill.
link |
01:35:02.540
It's not a sedative, which is really important.
link |
01:35:05.260
It's not that the mice are going to sleep.
link |
01:35:08.240
Most remarkably is once you socially isolate a mouse
link |
01:35:13.220
and it becomes aggressive,
link |
01:35:14.620
you can never put it back in its cage with its brothers
link |
01:35:18.960
from its litter because it will kill them all overnight.
link |
01:35:22.100
But if you give it this drug, which is called Osanatant,
link |
01:35:25.940
that blocks tachykinin II,
link |
01:35:29.700
that mouse can be returned to the cage with its brothers
link |
01:35:33.300
and will not attack them and seems to be happy about that
link |
01:35:37.340
for the rest of the time.
link |
01:35:38.700
So this is an incredibly powerful effect of this drug.
link |
01:35:42.540
And I've been really interested
link |
01:35:44.780
in trying to get pharmaceutical companies
link |
01:35:48.140
to test this drug,
link |
01:35:49.300
which has a really good safety profile in humans,
link |
01:35:52.980
in testing it in people who are subjected
link |
01:35:56.380
to social isolation stress or bereavement stress.
link |
01:36:00.620
And this is one of the areas
link |
01:36:02.980
where I learned an eye-opening lesson
link |
01:36:06.940
as a basic scientist who naively thought
link |
01:36:09.700
that if you make a discovery
link |
01:36:11.460
and it has translational applications to humans,
link |
01:36:15.220
that pharmaceutical companies are going to be falling
link |
01:36:17.740
all over themselves to try it.
link |
01:36:20.020
And they're not interested because once burned, twice shy.
link |
01:36:25.020
These drugs were tested for efficacy in schizophrenia.
link |
01:36:29.660
I have no idea why.
link |
01:36:31.700
There's very little preclinical data to suggest that.
link |
01:36:34.780
Not surprisingly, they failed.
link |
01:36:37.380
When a drug fails in clinical trials in phase three,
link |
01:36:41.940
it costs $100 million to the company
link |
01:36:45.940
that carried out that clinical trial.
link |
01:36:48.740
So there's a huge slag heap of discarded pharmaceuticals.
link |
01:36:53.500
Many of them inhibitors of neuropeptide action
link |
01:36:57.780
that could be useful in other indications,
link |
01:37:02.220
such as the one we discovered.
link |
01:37:04.100
But there's a huge economic disincentive
link |
01:37:07.500
for pharmaceutical companies to test them again,
link |
01:37:11.860
because the conclusion that they drew
link |
01:37:14.300
from all these failed tests,
link |
01:37:16.140
particularly in the 2010s and before that,
link |
01:37:20.220
is that the reason they failed
link |
01:37:22.740
is because animal experiments with drugs
link |
01:37:26.540
don't predict how humans will respond to the drugs.
link |
01:37:31.180
And therefore, we shouldn't try to extrapolate
link |
01:37:35.100
from any other data that we get from animal experiments,
link |
01:37:38.740
mouse or rat experiments, to humans,
link |
01:37:40.820
because they'll lead us down the wrong track.
link |
01:37:43.540
And I think that that is probably wrong.
link |
01:37:45.940
In some cases, it may be right,
link |
01:37:48.220
but in other cases, there's good reason to think,
link |
01:37:51.140
because these brain regions and molecules
link |
01:37:54.020
are so evolutionarily conserved
link |
01:37:56.740
that they ought to be playing a similar role in humans.
link |
01:38:00.500
In fact, there is a paper showing
link |
01:38:04.260
that in humans that have borderline personality disorder,
link |
01:38:09.780
there's a strong correlation
link |
01:38:11.460
between their self-reported level of aggressiveness
link |
01:38:15.500
and serum levels of a tachykinin,
link |
01:38:18.620
in this case, tachykinin-1,
link |
01:38:20.660
as detected by radioimmunoassay.
link |
01:38:22.860
This is work of Emil Coccaro,
link |
01:38:25.100
who's a clinical psychiatrist at the University of Chicago.
link |
01:38:28.620
So there is a smoking gun in the case of humans as well.
link |
01:38:33.220
And I was actually trying
link |
01:38:36.220
to interest a pharmaceutical company
link |
01:38:38.780
that was testing these drugs,
link |
01:38:41.700
actually for treatment of hot flashes in females, in humans,
link |
01:38:46.860
where there is actually good animal data
link |
01:38:49.860
to think that it might be useful.
link |
01:38:51.980
But I realized that this clinical trial
link |
01:38:55.140
was going on during the COVID pandemic.
link |
01:38:58.700
And I approached them and said,
link |
01:38:59.900
look, nature may have actually done for you
link |
01:39:02.860
the experiment that I want you to do,
link |
01:39:05.020
because some of the people who are getting drug or placebo
link |
01:39:08.140
are gonna have been socially isolated,
link |
01:39:10.620
and some of them will have not.
link |
01:39:12.140
Why don't you get them to fill out questionnaires
link |
01:39:14.780
and see whether the ones who are given the drug
link |
01:39:17.340
and socially isolated felt less stressed and less anxious
link |
01:39:21.540
than the ones who were not socially isolated,
link |
01:39:23.900
and they would not touch it
link |
01:39:26.180
because they're in the middle of a clinical trial
link |
01:39:29.300
for a different indication for this drug,
link |
01:39:31.820
and they have to report any observation
link |
01:39:35.220
that they make about that drug in their patient population.
link |
01:39:38.980
So if they were to ask these questions
link |
01:39:41.460
and get an unfavorable answer,
link |
01:39:44.060
oh my God, I felt even worse when I took this drug
link |
01:39:46.740
and I was isolated,
link |
01:39:47.980
they would be obliged to report that to the FDA,
link |
01:39:51.220
and that could torpedo the chances
link |
01:39:53.180
for the drug being approved
link |
01:39:54.820
in the thing that it was in clinical trials for.
link |
01:39:56.900
So it's better not to ask and not to know
link |
01:40:02.380
than it is to try to find out more information
link |
01:40:05.180
that could lead to another clinical indication.
link |
01:40:07.780
So I remain convinced that this family of drugs
link |
01:40:12.660
could have very powerful uses in treating some forms
link |
01:40:17.100
of stress-induced anxiety or aggressiveness in humans,
link |
01:40:21.260
but it's just very difficult for economic reasons
link |
01:40:24.420
to find a way to get somebody to test that.
link |
01:40:26.940
Yeah, a true shame that these companies won't do this,
link |
01:40:30.460
and especially given the fact
link |
01:40:32.300
that many of these drugs exist
link |
01:40:34.180
and their safety profiles are established,
link |
01:40:36.900
because that's always a serious consideration
link |
01:40:39.540
when embarking on a clinical trial.
link |
01:40:41.780
And perhaps in hearing this discussion,
link |
01:40:44.460
someone out there will understand
link |
01:40:46.340
the key importance of this and will reach out to us,
link |
01:40:49.620
will provide ways to do that
link |
01:40:52.300
to get such a study going in humans.
link |
01:40:55.140
Because I think if enough laboratories
link |
01:40:57.540
ran small-scale clinical trials,
link |
01:40:59.820
pharma certainly would perk up their ears, right?
link |
01:41:02.060
I mean, they're so strategic sometimes to their own.
link |
01:41:05.660
I mean, I would like to say also,
link |
01:41:07.540
I'd like to see this tested on pets.
link |
01:41:10.340
I mean, there's a huge number of pets right now
link |
01:41:13.060
that are suffering separation anxiety
link |
01:41:15.660
because humans bought them to keep them company
link |
01:41:18.540
during the COVID pandemic.
link |
01:41:19.900
And now they're home alone.
link |
01:41:20.740
And now they're home alone, okay?
link |
01:41:22.420
And if this thing works in mice,
link |
01:41:24.540
there's certainly a higher chance
link |
01:41:26.380
it's gonna work in dogs or in cats
link |
01:41:28.980
than it is gonna work in humans.
link |
01:41:30.780
And if it did, that would be even more encouragement
link |
01:41:33.740
to continue along those lines.
link |
01:41:35.660
People sometimes forget that although we work on animals
link |
01:41:39.180
and we ultimately wanna understand humans,
link |
01:41:41.500
we care about how our results apply
link |
01:41:44.220
to the welfare of animals as well,
link |
01:41:46.740
and particularly domestic pets,
link |
01:41:48.780
which is a billion, multi-billion dollar industry
link |
01:41:51.980
in this country.
link |
01:41:52.900
So if there's ways that they can be made to feel better
link |
01:41:56.300
when they're separated from their owners,
link |
01:41:59.500
that would certainly be a good thing.
link |
01:42:01.300
Absolutely.
link |
01:42:02.780
We will put out the call.
link |
01:42:03.820
We are putting out the call.
link |
01:42:05.060
And I know for sure there will be a response.
link |
01:42:10.540
Just underscoring what we've been talking about even more,
link |
01:42:13.420
every time we hear about a school shooting,
link |
01:42:16.740
like in Texas recently,
link |
01:42:18.060
or I happened to be in New York during the time
link |
01:42:20.300
when there was a subway shooting,
link |
01:42:23.020
for whatever reason, I listened to the book about Columbine
link |
01:42:28.140
that went into a very detailed way
link |
01:42:30.020
about the origin of those boys and that committed that.
link |
01:42:33.260
And every single time the person who commits those acts
link |
01:42:39.180
is socially isolated, as far as I know.
link |
01:42:41.460
There might be some exceptions there.
link |
01:42:42.980
And sometimes this crosses over
link |
01:42:44.300
with other mental health issues, but sometimes no,
link |
01:42:46.460
no apparent mental health issues.
link |
01:42:48.180
So social isolation clearly drives powerful neurochemical
link |
01:42:52.420
and neurobiological changes.
link |
01:42:54.300
I really hope that tachykinin 1 and 2,
link |
01:42:57.020
those are the main ones in humans,
link |
01:42:58.860
will be explored in more detail.
link |
01:43:01.420
Also, I didn't know that tachykinin 1
link |
01:43:02.940
is substance P and substance P is tachykinin 1.
link |
01:43:05.860
Tachykinin 1 is the gene name
link |
01:43:08.060
and tachykinin 2 in humans is called neurokinin B.
link |
01:43:12.260
That's the name of the protein.
link |
01:43:14.180
I just referred to it by the gene name
link |
01:43:16.100
because it makes it easier
link |
01:43:17.300
and I don't have to keep remembering
link |
01:43:18.780
two names for each thing.
link |
01:43:20.300
And I, if I'm not mistaken,
link |
01:43:22.060
you put yourself in the company of geneticists
link |
01:43:25.540
because of your original training
link |
01:43:26.820
was in genetics, immunology and areas related to that.
link |
01:43:29.860
It was in cell biology
link |
01:43:31.540
and I didn't actually have formal training in genetics
link |
01:43:34.700
as a graduate student,
link |
01:43:35.900
but I think I'm a geneticist at heart.
link |
01:43:38.180
That's just the way I like to think about things.
link |
01:43:41.300
And when I started working on flies,
link |
01:43:43.420
that sort of, I came out of the closet
link |
01:43:46.020
as a geneticist, as it were.
link |
01:43:48.220
Wonderful.
link |
01:43:49.660
As long as we're talking about humans,
link |
01:43:50.900
I'd love to get your thoughts
link |
01:43:51.780
about human studies of emotion.
link |
01:43:53.200
I know you wrote this book with Ralph Adol.
link |
01:43:55.140
So you have this new book,
link |
01:43:56.220
which we'll provide a link to,
link |
01:43:57.380
which I've read front to back twice.
link |
01:43:59.840
It's phenomenal.
link |
01:44:01.020
I've mentioned it before on the podcast.
link |
01:44:02.460
It's really, there are books that are worth reading
link |
01:44:05.140
and then there are books that are important.
link |
01:44:06.620
And I think this book is truly important
link |
01:44:08.220
for the general population to read and understand.
link |
01:44:10.800
And neuroscientists should read and understand the contents
link |
01:44:13.220
because we, as a culture,
link |
01:44:17.060
are way off in terms of how we think about emotions
link |
01:44:19.500
and states and behaviors.
link |
01:44:21.340
So we'll put a link to that.
link |
01:44:22.460
It's really worth the time and energy to read it.
link |
01:44:25.880
And it's written beautifully, I should say.
link |
01:44:27.520
Very accessible, even for non-scientists.
link |
01:44:30.280
There's a heat map diagram in that book that I think about.
link |
01:44:35.580
This is a heat map diagram of subjective reports
link |
01:44:38.660
that people gave of where they experience an emotion
link |
01:44:43.740
or a feeling, a somatic feeling in their body
link |
01:44:46.540
or in their head or both when they are angry, sad, calm,
link |
01:44:51.140
lonely, et cetera, et cetera.
link |
01:44:53.820
And I wouldn't want people to think that those heat maps
link |
01:44:56.520
were generated by any physiological measurement
link |
01:45:00.900
because they were not.
link |
01:45:01.860
And yet, I don't think we can have a discussion
link |
01:45:04.900
about emotions and states and the sorts of behaviors
link |
01:45:08.340
that we're talking about today
link |
01:45:09.180
without thinking about the body also.
link |
01:45:11.720
And I'm not coming to this
link |
01:45:12.860
as a Northern California mind body.
link |
01:45:14.940
I've been to Esalen once.
link |
01:45:16.260
I didn't go in the baths.
link |
01:45:17.420
I went there, I gave a talk and I left.
link |
01:45:18.980
It is very beautiful.
link |
01:45:20.500
If anyone wants to know what it looks like,
link |
01:45:21.660
I think that final scene of Mad Men is shot at Esalen.
link |
01:45:24.700
It's a very beautiful place.
link |
01:45:26.620
And yet, mind body to me is a neurobiological construct
link |
01:45:31.820
because the nervous system extends
link |
01:45:33.180
through the out of the cranial vault
link |
01:45:35.100
and into the spinal cord and body and back and forth.
link |
01:45:37.340
Okay, how should we think about the body
link |
01:45:41.220
and in terms of states?
link |
01:45:43.460
And at some point, I'd love for you to comment
link |
01:45:45.760
on that heat map experiment
link |
01:45:47.580
because it does seem that there's some regularity
link |
01:45:50.280
as to where people experience emotions.
link |
01:45:52.860
When people are in a rage, for instance,
link |
01:45:54.580
they seem to feel it both in their gut and in their head,
link |
01:45:58.100
it seems, on average.
link |
01:46:00.580
And people love to extrapolate to gut intuition
link |
01:46:05.300
or that the chakras or anger is in the stomach
link |
01:46:08.740
and this goes to Eastern medicine, et cetera.
link |
01:46:11.780
How should we think about mind body in the context of states
link |
01:46:14.980
and think about it as scientists,
link |
01:46:16.780
maybe even as neuroscientists or geneticists?
link |
01:46:20.060
Good.
link |
01:46:21.140
So for the answer to the first question about the heat maps
link |
01:46:25.160
and people associating certain parts of their body
link |
01:46:28.460
with certain emotional feelings,
link |
01:46:30.880
this goes back to something called
link |
01:46:33.420
the somatic marker hypothesis
link |
01:46:35.660
that was proposed by Antonio Damasio,
link |
01:46:38.340
who is a neurologist at USC.
link |
01:46:41.380
The idea that our subjective feeling
link |
01:46:45.500
of a particular emotion is in part associated
link |
01:46:50.500
with a sensation of something happening
link |
01:46:55.140
in a particular part of our body.
link |
01:46:57.500
The gut, the heart, I don't see the liver invoked very much
link |
01:47:02.740
in emotional characterization.
link |
01:47:06.700
But gall and the gallbladder?
link |
01:47:08.260
Yes.
link |
01:47:09.340
Somebody having a lot of gall.
link |
01:47:10.180
That's right.
link |
01:47:11.020
I don't know why I make a fist when I say that,
link |
01:47:12.100
but I'm guessing the gallbladder is shaped like a fist.
link |
01:47:14.120
That's right.
link |
01:47:16.060
And if there is a physiology underlying these heat maps,
link |
01:47:20.260
it could reflect increased blood flow
link |
01:47:22.860
to these different structures.
link |
01:47:24.380
And that in turn reflects what you were talking about,
link |
01:47:28.060
that is emotion definitely involves communication
link |
01:47:31.800
between the brain and the body,
link |
01:47:33.780
and it's bi-directional communication.
link |
01:47:36.900
And it's mediated by the peripheral nervous system,
link |
01:47:41.280
the sympathetic and the parasympathetic nervous system,
link |
01:47:44.520
which control heart rate, for example,
link |
01:47:47.460
blood vessel, blood pressure,
link |
01:47:50.220
and those neurons receive input from the hypothalamus
link |
01:47:54.740
and other blood brain regions, central brain regions,
link |
01:47:58.380
that control their activity.
link |
01:48:00.380
And when the brain is put in a particular state,
link |
01:48:05.040
it activates sympathetic and parasympathetic neurons,
link |
01:48:09.020
which have effects on the heart and on blood pressure.
link |
01:48:13.780
And these in turn feed back onto the brain
link |
01:48:18.140
through the sensory system.
link |
01:48:20.040
And a large part of this bi-directional communication
link |
01:48:23.860
is also mediated through the vagus nerve,
link |
01:48:27.220
which many of your listeners and viewers
link |
01:48:30.860
may have heard about,
link |
01:48:31.700
because it's become a topic of intense activity now.
link |
01:48:35.700
People have known for a long time.
link |
01:48:38.340
So the vagus nerve is a bundle of nerve fibers
link |
01:48:43.560
that comes out basically of your skull,
link |
01:48:47.300
out of the central nervous system,
link |
01:48:49.460
and then sends fibers in to your heart,
link |
01:48:54.500
your gut, all sorts of visceral organs.
link |
01:48:58.180
So when you have, and that information is both,
link |
01:49:01.460
you used the words earlier in our discussion,
link |
01:49:04.740
afferent and efferent.
link |
01:49:06.940
So the vagal fibers sense things
link |
01:49:11.740
that are happening in the body.
link |
01:49:13.860
So when you're, the reason you feel your stomach
link |
01:49:16.940
hide up in knots if you're tense
link |
01:49:19.580
is that those vagal fibers are sensing
link |
01:49:22.980
the contraction of the gut muscles.
link |
01:49:25.900
And they're also afferents,
link |
01:49:27.780
which means that information coming out of the brain
link |
01:49:31.140
can influence those peripheral organs as well.
link |
01:49:34.780
And there's work from a number of labs
link |
01:49:37.100
just in the last six months or so
link |
01:49:40.380
where people are starting to decode
link |
01:49:43.420
the components of the different fibers in the vagus nerve.
link |
01:49:48.260
And it's amazing how much specificity is.
link |
01:49:51.240
There are specific vagal nerves that go to the lung,
link |
01:49:55.360
that control breathing responses,
link |
01:49:57.620
that go to the gut, that go to other organs.
link |
01:50:01.620
It's almost like a set of color-coded lines,
link |
01:50:06.260
labeled lines for those things.
link |
01:50:08.120
And now how those vagal afferents play a role
link |
01:50:13.460
in the playing out of emotion states
link |
01:50:16.600
is a fascinating question that people
link |
01:50:18.900
are just beginning to scrape the surface of.
link |
01:50:21.700
But I think what's exciting now
link |
01:50:23.820
is that people are gonna be developing tools
link |
01:50:25.980
that will allow us to turn on or turn off
link |
01:50:28.940
specific subsets of fibers within the vagus nerve
link |
01:50:33.380
and ask how that affects particular emotional behaviors.
link |
01:50:37.060
So you're absolutely right.
link |
01:50:38.680
This brain-body connection is critical,
link |
01:50:41.340
not just for the gut, but for the heart, for the lungs,
link |
01:50:45.020
for all kinds of other parts of your body.
link |
01:50:48.060
And Darwin recognized that as well.
link |
01:50:50.540
And I think it's a central feature of emotion state.
link |
01:50:54.680
And I think what underlies
link |
01:50:56.380
are subjective feelings of an emotion.
link |
01:50:59.860
Incredible.
link |
01:51:01.420
David, I have to say, as a true fan of the work
link |
01:51:04.300
that your lab has been doing over so many decades,
link |
01:51:07.060
and first of all, I was delighted
link |
01:51:09.140
when you stopped working on stem cells,
link |
01:51:10.620
not because you weren't doing incredible work there,
link |
01:51:12.540
but because I saw a talk where you showed a movie
link |
01:51:16.100
of an octopus spitting out, or not spitting,
link |
01:51:19.860
but squirting out a bunch of ink and escaping.
link |
01:51:22.340
And you said you were gonna work on things of the sort
link |
01:51:24.660
that we're talking about today,
link |
01:51:25.540
fear, aggression, mating behaviors, social behaviors.
link |
01:51:29.060
It's been incredible to see the work that your lab has done.
link |
01:51:31.420
And I know I speak on behalf of a tremendous number of people
link |
01:51:37.100
when I say thank you for taking time
link |
01:51:39.220
out of your important schedule
link |
01:51:40.740
to share with us what you've learned.
link |
01:51:42.680
My last question is a simple one,
link |
01:51:45.460
which is will you come back and talk to us again
link |
01:51:48.340
in the future about the additional work that's sure to come?
link |
01:51:50.820
I would be happy to do that.
link |
01:51:52.300
And I really have appreciated your questions.
link |
01:51:55.700
They've all been right on the money.
link |
01:51:57.460
You've hit all of the critical,
link |
01:51:59.620
important issues in this field,
link |
01:52:01.860
and you've uncovered what is known,
link |
01:52:05.620
the little bit is known, and how much is not known.
link |
01:52:08.820
And I think it's important to emphasize the unknown things
link |
01:52:13.100
because that's what the next generation
link |
01:52:15.420
of neuroscientists has to solve.
link |
01:52:18.100
And so I hope this will help to attract young people
link |
01:52:21.140
into this field because it's so important,
link |
01:52:24.300
particularly for our understanding of mental illness
link |
01:52:27.620
and mental health and psychiatry,
link |
01:52:31.700
we've got to figure out how emotion systems are controlled
link |
01:52:36.060
in a causal way if we ever want to improve
link |
01:52:39.920
on the psychiatric treatments that we have now.
link |
01:52:42.580
And that's gonna require the next generation
link |
01:52:44.980
of people coming into the field.
link |
01:52:46.780
Absolutely, I second that.
link |
01:52:48.860
Well, thank you, it's been a delight.
link |
01:52:50.380
Thank you, great, really appreciate it.
link |
01:52:53.500
Thank you for joining me today
link |
01:52:54.620
for my discussion with Dr. David Anderson.
link |
01:52:56.680
Please also be sure to check out his new book,
link |
01:52:58.940
The Nature of the Beast, How Emotions Guide Us.
link |
01:53:01.860
It's a truly masterful exploration of the biology
link |
01:53:04.680
and psychology behind what we call emotions
link |
01:53:07.180
and states of mind and body.
link |
01:53:09.060
If you're learning from and are enjoying this podcast,
link |
01:53:11.460
please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
link |
01:53:13.460
That's a simple zero cost way to support us.
link |
01:53:15.880
Please also subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple.
link |
01:53:19.140
And on both Spotify and Apple,
link |
01:53:21.020
you have the opportunity to leave us
link |
01:53:22.440
up to a five-star review.
link |
01:53:24.300
If you have questions or comments or suggestions
link |
01:53:26.320
about topics you'd like us to cover,
link |
01:53:27.820
or guests you'd like us to interview
link |
01:53:29.060
on the Huberman Lab podcast,
link |
01:53:30.700
please put those in the comment section on YouTube.
link |
01:53:33.380
We do read all those comments and we do take them to heart.
link |
01:53:36.320
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
link |
01:53:37.940
at the beginning of today's podcast.
link |
01:53:39.820
And check out Momentous Supplements,
link |
01:53:41.680
our new partners in the supplement space,
link |
01:53:43.340
and check out Athletic Greens.
link |
01:53:45.360
That's the best way to support this podcast.
link |
01:53:47.660
If you're not already following us on social media,
link |
01:53:49.900
please do so.
link |
01:53:51.220
We are Huberman Lab on Twitter,
link |
01:53:52.980
and we are also Huberman Lab on Instagram.
link |
01:53:55.000
In both places, I cover science and science-related tools,
link |
01:53:58.340
some of which overlap with the content
link |
01:53:59.900
of the Huberman Lab podcast,
link |
01:54:01.060
but much of which is unique from the content
link |
01:54:03.300
covered on the Huberman Lab podcast.
link |
01:54:04.820
Again, that's Huberman Lab on Instagram
link |
01:54:06.540
and Huberman Lab on Twitter.
link |
01:54:08.180
Please also check out our Neural Network monthly newsletter.
link |
01:54:11.100
This is a newsletter that has summaries of podcast episodes.
link |
01:54:14.580
It also includes a lot of actionable protocols.
link |
01:54:16.860
It's very easy to sign up for the newsletter.
link |
01:54:18.920
You go to HubermanLab.com, click on the menu,
link |
01:54:21.080
go to newsletter, you supply your email,
link |
01:54:23.140
but we do not share your email with anybody.
link |
01:54:25.140
We have a very clear and rigorous privacy policy,
link |
01:54:28.060
which is we do not share your email with anybody.
link |
01:54:30.260
And the newsletter comes out once a month,
link |
01:54:32.780
and it is completely zero cost.
link |
01:54:34.300
Again, just go to HubermanLab.com
link |
01:54:36.340
and go to the Neural Network newsletter.
link |
01:54:38.800
I'd also like to point out that the Huberman Lab podcast
link |
01:54:41.120
has a clips channel, so these are brief clips,
link |
01:54:44.620
anywhere from three to 10 minutes
link |
01:54:46.660
that encompass single concepts and actionable protocols
link |
01:54:50.240
related to sleep, to focus,
link |
01:54:52.140
interviews with various guests.
link |
01:54:53.420
We talk about things like caffeine,
link |
01:54:54.800
when to drink caffeine relative to sleep, alcohol,
link |
01:54:57.920
when and how and if anyone should ingest it
link |
01:54:59.880
relative to sleep, dopamine, serotonin, mental health,
link |
01:55:03.540
physical health, and on and on,
link |
01:55:05.140
all the things that relate to the topics
link |
01:55:07.100
most of interest to you.
link |
01:55:08.440
You can find that easily by going to YouTube,
link |
01:55:11.100
look for Huberman Lab clips in the search area,
link |
01:55:13.980
and it will take you there, subscribe,
link |
01:55:15.580
and we are constantly updating those with new clips.
link |
01:55:17.920
This is especially useful, I believe,
link |
01:55:19.460
for people that have missed some of the earlier episodes
link |
01:55:21.500
or you're still working through the back catalog
link |
01:55:23.060
of Huberman Lab podcasts,
link |
01:55:24.140
which admittedly can be rather long.
link |
01:55:26.260
And last, but certainly not least,
link |
01:55:28.580
thank you for your interest in science.
link |
01:55:30.420
I'll see you in the next one.