back to index

Science of Social Bonding in Family, Friendship & Romantic Love | Huberman Lab Podcast #51



link |
00:00:00.000
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
link |
00:00:02.280
where we discuss science and science-based tools
link |
00:00:04.880
for everyday life.
link |
00:00:09.160
I'm Andrew Huberman,
link |
00:00:10.320
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
link |
00:00:12.960
at Stanford School of Medicine.
link |
00:00:14.880
Today's episode is about the biology, psychology,
link |
00:00:18.000
and practices of social bonding.
link |
00:00:20.720
From the day we are born until the day we die,
link |
00:00:23.700
the quality of our social bonds
link |
00:00:25.400
dictates much of our quality of life.
link |
00:00:28.240
It should therefore be no surprise that our brain,
link |
00:00:31.440
and indeed much of our entire nervous system,
link |
00:00:34.120
is wired for social bonds.
link |
00:00:36.780
Now, social bonds occur between infant and parent.
link |
00:00:40.360
There are even particular wiring diagrams
link |
00:00:43.040
within the brain and spinal cord and body
link |
00:00:45.760
that are oriented towards the specific bonds
link |
00:00:48.080
that occur between infant and mother,
link |
00:00:50.360
as well as infant and father.
link |
00:00:52.860
And we have specific brain circuitries for friendship,
link |
00:00:56.180
specific brain circuitries that are activated
link |
00:00:58.780
in romantic relationships.
link |
00:01:00.560
And as it goes, specific brain circuitries
link |
00:01:03.580
that are activated when we break up with a romantic partner,
link |
00:01:06.940
or when they break up with us,
link |
00:01:08.380
or when somebody passes away, moves away,
link |
00:01:11.200
or otherwise leaves our lives in one form or another.
link |
00:01:15.520
Today, we are going to talk about
link |
00:01:17.040
those brain and nervous system circuitries.
link |
00:01:19.440
We're also going to talk about the neurochemicals
link |
00:01:21.400
and hormones that underlie their function.
link |
00:01:24.240
And we are going to touch on a number of important
link |
00:01:26.880
and actionable tools that you can apply in everyday life.
link |
00:01:30.560
And because we are headed into the holiday,
link |
00:01:33.120
the New Year's and Christmas holiday,
link |
00:01:35.840
that you can deploy in your various interactions
link |
00:01:38.300
with family members and friends.
link |
00:01:40.040
And should you not be spending time
link |
00:01:42.020
with family members and friends,
link |
00:01:43.120
today we are also going to talk about
link |
00:01:44.900
how to achieve social bonds out of the context
link |
00:01:48.260
of family and romantic partnership and friendship.
link |
00:01:51.320
So today's episode is going to include a lot of science,
link |
00:01:54.120
a lot of actionable tools,
link |
00:01:55.800
and I'm confident that you will come away
link |
00:01:57.760
from today's episode with tremendous knowledge
link |
00:02:00.700
about how you function.
link |
00:02:02.580
For instance, if you're an introvert or an extrovert,
link |
00:02:05.000
why is that?
link |
00:02:05.840
Turns out there may be a neurochemical basis for that.
link |
00:02:08.240
Maybe you're somebody that really enjoys social media.
link |
00:02:10.320
Maybe you're somebody that doesn't.
link |
00:02:11.680
Today, I'm going to talk about a gene or a set of genes
link |
00:02:14.680
that predicts whether or not you will follow more people
link |
00:02:17.560
or seek out more online social interactions or fewer.
link |
00:02:21.640
Believe it or not, there's biology around that now,
link |
00:02:23.720
and it's excellent peer reviewed work.
link |
00:02:25.760
We will also talk about how bonds are broken
link |
00:02:28.960
and why breakups can be so painful,
link |
00:02:31.220
not just romantic breakups,
link |
00:02:32.500
but breakups with friendships and coworkers
link |
00:02:35.160
and how to move through those more seamlessly.
link |
00:02:38.080
So regardless of your age and regardless of whether or not
link |
00:02:40.840
you are in a romantic partnership of one form or another
link |
00:02:43.760
or not, I do believe this episode will be useful to you
link |
00:02:47.280
as you explore the social bonds
link |
00:02:49.380
that already exist in your life
link |
00:02:50.800
and as you seek out new and changing social bonds.
link |
00:02:55.160
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
link |
00:02:57.560
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
link |
00:03:00.120
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
link |
00:03:02.040
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
link |
00:03:04.560
and science-related tools to the general public.
link |
00:03:07.180
In keeping with that theme,
link |
00:03:08.120
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
link |
00:03:10.680
Our first sponsor is Roca.
link |
00:03:12.380
Roca makes eyeglasses and sunglasses
link |
00:03:14.360
that are the absolute highest quality.
link |
00:03:16.860
I've spent a lifetime working on the biology
link |
00:03:18.520
of the visual system,
link |
00:03:19.440
and the visual system has many important features
link |
00:03:21.660
that allow us, for instance,
link |
00:03:22.740
to see things with crystal clarity,
link |
00:03:24.580
even if it's brightly lit or dimly lit
link |
00:03:26.800
or they're changing brightness in our environment.
link |
00:03:30.060
Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses are designed
link |
00:03:32.860
with the biology of the visual system in mind.
link |
00:03:35.440
So when you wear them,
link |
00:03:36.480
you don't have to take them off and put them back on
link |
00:03:38.520
like you would with other types of sunglasses and eyeglasses,
link |
00:03:41.000
and you're always going to see things with crystal clarity.
link |
00:03:43.640
In addition, they're very lightweight,
link |
00:03:45.280
so usually you forget that you're wearing them.
link |
00:03:46.680
I certainly forget when I'm wearing mine.
link |
00:03:48.080
I wear readers at night and when I drive
link |
00:03:49.920
and I wear sunglasses often throughout the day.
link |
00:03:52.480
The other thing that's really great
link |
00:03:53.460
about Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses
link |
00:03:55.400
is that you can wear them doing anything.
link |
00:03:58.040
You can wear them while running, while cycling.
link |
00:04:00.640
If you get sweaty, they won't fall off your face.
link |
00:04:03.080
But unlike a lot of other performance eyeglasses out there,
link |
00:04:06.660
they have a terrific aesthetic
link |
00:04:07.800
that you could also wear to work, to school, out to dinner.
link |
00:04:11.120
They're basically going to be worn anywhere, anytime.
link |
00:04:13.580
If you'd like to try Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses,
link |
00:04:15.960
you can go to Roca, that's R-O-K-A.com
link |
00:04:18.600
and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your first order.
link |
00:04:22.000
Again, that's Roca, R-O-K-A.com
link |
00:04:24.080
and enter the code Huberman at checkout to get 20% off.
link |
00:04:27.580
Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens.
link |
00:04:30.280
Athletic Greens is an all-in-one
link |
00:04:31.760
vitamin mineral probiotic drink.
link |
00:04:34.040
I started taking Athletic Greens way back in 2012
link |
00:04:37.360
and I've taken it ever since,
link |
00:04:38.500
so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
link |
00:04:40.920
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens
link |
00:04:42.620
and the reason that I still take Athletic Greens
link |
00:04:44.340
once or twice a day is because it covers
link |
00:04:46.840
all of my vitamin mineral probiotic foundational needs.
link |
00:04:50.640
There's now a wealth of evidence pointing to the fact
link |
00:04:53.000
that vitamins, minerals, and probiotics
link |
00:04:54.760
support a healthy gut microbiome, healthy immune system,
link |
00:04:57.880
healthy nervous system, healthy endocrine system,
link |
00:05:00.160
that's the hormone system.
link |
00:05:01.360
And with Athletic Greens, it really covers all those bases.
link |
00:05:04.200
In fact, when people ask me
link |
00:05:05.600
what one supplement they should take,
link |
00:05:07.360
I recommend Athletic Greens.
link |
00:05:09.240
If you'd like to try Athletic Greens,
link |
00:05:10.560
you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman.
link |
00:05:13.140
And if you do that, you can claim a special offer.
link |
00:05:15.840
They're giving five free travel packs,
link |
00:05:17.540
which make it very easy to mix up the Athletic Greens
link |
00:05:19.540
while on the road, in the car, on the plane, et cetera,
link |
00:05:21.960
plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
link |
00:05:25.320
There's now a lot of evidence supporting that vitamin D3
link |
00:05:29.120
is great for our metabolism, endocrine system, mood, sleep,
link |
00:05:33.000
a huge number of biological functions.
link |
00:05:34.720
And K2 has also been shown to be important
link |
00:05:37.060
for cardiovascular health and various aspects
link |
00:05:39.320
of blood lipids.
link |
00:05:40.200
So again, if you go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman,
link |
00:05:42.920
you can claim a special offer.
link |
00:05:44.300
You'll get your Athletic Greens,
link |
00:05:45.360
five free travel packs of Athletic Greens,
link |
00:05:47.280
plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
link |
00:05:50.480
Today's episode is also brought to us by Headspace.
link |
00:05:53.200
Headspace is a meditation app
link |
00:05:54.540
supported by 25 published studies
link |
00:05:56.520
and has over 600,000 five-star reviews.
link |
00:06:00.040
I've been meditating for a very long time,
link |
00:06:02.040
more than three decades, but I confess,
link |
00:06:04.680
I haven't been a consistent meditator until recently.
link |
00:06:07.920
I think many people have that experience.
link |
00:06:09.860
We've all heard about the terrific science
link |
00:06:12.000
and benefits of a regular meditation practice,
link |
00:06:14.360
things like reduced anxiety, improved sleep,
link |
00:06:17.020
sharper cognition and focus.
link |
00:06:19.240
However, sticking to a meditation practice can be tricky
link |
00:06:22.480
and many people start and then they stop,
link |
00:06:24.360
or at least that's what happened to me
link |
00:06:25.480
until a few years ago when I started using
link |
00:06:27.080
the Headspace app to do my meditation.
link |
00:06:30.160
Headspace has short meditations
link |
00:06:32.000
of just a couple minutes long, longer meditations.
link |
00:06:34.680
Ideally for me, I like to do a 20-minute meditation
link |
00:06:37.680
at least three times a week,
link |
00:06:38.840
but sometimes because of my schedule
link |
00:06:40.560
and other things going on,
link |
00:06:41.680
I only sneak in a five-minute meditation
link |
00:06:43.560
once or twice a week.
link |
00:06:45.320
With Headspace, you have a huge number of meditations
link |
00:06:48.240
to select from.
link |
00:06:49.320
So if you want to try Headspace,
link |
00:06:50.660
you can go to headspace.com slash special offer
link |
00:06:53.440
and if you do that, you'll get a completely free
link |
00:06:55.640
one-month trial with all of Headspace's meditations.
link |
00:06:58.840
That's the full library of meditations
link |
00:07:00.620
that Headspace has to offer.
link |
00:07:02.140
That's the best deal offered by Headspace right now.
link |
00:07:04.000
So again, if you're interested,
link |
00:07:05.440
go to headspace.com slash special offer today.
link |
00:07:08.520
Let's talk about the biology of social bonding
link |
00:07:10.880
and I want to point out
link |
00:07:11.800
that I use the word bonding intentionally.
link |
00:07:14.840
It's a verb and in biology, we want to think about verbs
link |
00:07:18.960
because everything in biology is a process.
link |
00:07:22.280
It's not an event.
link |
00:07:24.000
And when we think about things in biology as a process,
link |
00:07:26.480
that means it's going to have multiple steps
link |
00:07:28.360
and today we are going to explore the steps
link |
00:07:30.880
start to finish of social bonding,
link |
00:07:33.380
meaning how social bonds are established,
link |
00:07:35.700
how they are maintained, how they are broken
link |
00:07:38.020
and how they are reestablished.
link |
00:07:40.360
Now an important feature of biology generally,
link |
00:07:42.880
but in particular, as it relates to social bonding
link |
00:07:45.760
is that the neural circuits,
link |
00:07:48.560
meaning the brain areas and neurons and the hormones,
link |
00:07:51.580
things like oxytocin, which we'll talk about today
link |
00:07:54.360
and the other chemicals in the brain and body
link |
00:07:57.000
that are responsible for the process we call social bonding
link |
00:08:00.240
are not unique to particular social bonds.
link |
00:08:04.020
They are generic.
link |
00:08:05.360
What I mean by that is that the same brain circuits
link |
00:08:08.000
that are responsible for establishing a bond
link |
00:08:10.600
between parent and child are actually repurposed
link |
00:08:14.220
in romantic relationships.
link |
00:08:16.160
And this might not come as a surprise to many of you.
link |
00:08:18.640
Many of you are probably familiar with this idea
link |
00:08:20.620
of securely attached people versus anxious attached people
link |
00:08:23.880
versus avoidant attached people.
link |
00:08:25.640
We're going to touch on that a little bit,
link |
00:08:27.400
but all of that has roots in whether or not children
link |
00:08:30.480
and parents formed healthy social bonds
link |
00:08:34.120
or whether or not they had challenged social bonds.
link |
00:08:37.360
Now it's clear from the scientific
link |
00:08:40.200
and psychological literature
link |
00:08:42.520
that just because you might've had a not so great
link |
00:08:46.000
or even terrible social bond with a parent
link |
00:08:49.100
or with some other caretaker or loved one as a child,
link |
00:08:52.240
that doesn't fate you to have poor social bonds as an adult.
link |
00:08:55.180
There's a lot of plasticity in the system,
link |
00:08:57.540
meaning it can change,
link |
00:08:58.960
it can rewire in response to experience.
link |
00:09:01.480
And as we will soon discover,
link |
00:09:04.140
there are specific components within the neural circuits
link |
00:09:07.240
of your brain that are responsible for social bonding
link |
00:09:10.320
that allow you to place subjective labels
link |
00:09:13.520
on why you are doing certain things
link |
00:09:15.320
and to rewire the neural circuits for social bonding.
link |
00:09:18.020
So we're going to touch on all of that today.
link |
00:09:19.920
But the important feature really to point out
link |
00:09:22.540
is that we don't have 12 different circuits
link |
00:09:25.120
in the brain and body for different types of social bonds.
link |
00:09:27.820
We have one, and there's some universal features
link |
00:09:30.840
that underlie all forms of social bonds.
link |
00:09:33.640
So we're going to start by exploring
link |
00:09:35.240
what those neural circuits are.
link |
00:09:37.000
And then we are going to see how they plug in
link |
00:09:39.240
to different types of social bonds.
link |
00:09:41.280
And then we are going to explore things like introversion,
link |
00:09:44.880
extraversion, where you're going to touch on a little bit
link |
00:09:47.560
about things like trauma bonds, healthy bonds,
link |
00:09:50.360
and various other aspects
link |
00:09:52.240
of how humans can bond to one another.
link |
00:09:54.600
And as you'll soon discover,
link |
00:09:56.280
there is a unique chemical signature
link |
00:09:58.760
of all bonding of all kinds.
link |
00:10:01.200
And you're going to learn
link |
00:10:02.040
how to modulate that chemical signature.
link |
00:10:04.460
Before we talk about social bonding,
link |
00:10:06.080
I want to talk about its mirror image,
link |
00:10:08.180
which is lack of social bonding or social isolation.
link |
00:10:11.680
Now, for better or for worse,
link |
00:10:13.860
there is a tremendous literature
link |
00:10:16.520
on the biology of social isolation
link |
00:10:19.340
and all of the terrible things that happen
link |
00:10:21.120
when animals or humans are socially isolated
link |
00:10:24.160
at particular phases of life.
link |
00:10:26.740
Now, for those of you that are introverts,
link |
00:10:29.080
you are not necessarily damaging yourself
link |
00:10:31.080
by deciding to spend less time with other people.
link |
00:10:33.080
Many people like time alone.
link |
00:10:34.200
I personally am an introvert.
link |
00:10:36.080
I get a thrill out of spending time
link |
00:10:38.300
with one or two close friends,
link |
00:10:40.360
but I enjoy a lot of time by myself.
link |
00:10:42.540
I like to socialize,
link |
00:10:43.600
so I wouldn't call myself an extreme introvert,
link |
00:10:45.640
but I know there are some extreme introverts out there.
link |
00:10:47.880
But when we talk about social isolation,
link |
00:10:49.680
what we're referring to is when animals or humans
link |
00:10:54.000
are restricted from having the social contacts
link |
00:10:56.980
that they would prefer to have.
link |
00:10:59.160
And to just briefly touch on the major takeaways
link |
00:11:02.480
from this literature, which spans back 100 years or more,
link |
00:11:07.680
being socially isolated is stressful.
link |
00:11:10.460
And one of the hallmark features of social isolation
link |
00:11:13.480
is chronically elevated stress hormones,
link |
00:11:16.060
like adrenaline, also called epinephrine,
link |
00:11:18.040
like cortisol, a stress hormone that at healthy levels
link |
00:11:21.680
is good for combating inflammation,
link |
00:11:24.020
helps us have energy early in the day,
link |
00:11:25.960
focused throughout the day.
link |
00:11:27.300
But if cortisol is elevated for too long,
link |
00:11:30.240
which is the consequence of social isolation,
link |
00:11:33.140
the immune system suffers and other chemicals
link |
00:11:36.240
start to be released in the brain and body
link |
00:11:38.360
that are designed to motivate the organism,
link |
00:11:40.780
animal or human, to seek out social bonds.
link |
00:11:44.800
An example of one such chemical
link |
00:11:46.560
is a peptide called tachykinin.
link |
00:11:48.840
Tachykinin is present in flies, in mice, and in humans,
link |
00:11:53.720
and under conditions of social isolation,
link |
00:11:56.460
its levels go up, and because of the brain areas
link |
00:11:59.760
that contain receptors for tachykinin,
link |
00:12:02.400
people start feeling very aggressive and irritable
link |
00:12:06.120
after social isolation.
link |
00:12:07.920
Now, that should be a little bit counterintuitive to you.
link |
00:12:10.040
You would think, oh, if you isolate an animal or human,
link |
00:12:13.160
and then you give them the opportunity
link |
00:12:14.820
for social interaction, they should behave very well,
link |
00:12:17.280
they should be thrilled,
link |
00:12:18.120
they're finally getting the nourishment,
link |
00:12:19.960
the social nourishment that they've been lacking
link |
00:12:21.900
for so long, it turns out that's not the case.
link |
00:12:24.200
Chronic social isolation changes the nature
link |
00:12:27.560
of the brain and body such that it makes
link |
00:12:29.760
social connection harder,
link |
00:12:31.760
and it makes the person who's been isolated irritable,
link |
00:12:35.000
even aggressive with other people.
link |
00:12:37.560
Now, I don't want to go too deeply
link |
00:12:38.900
into the biology of social isolation,
link |
00:12:41.580
because it doesn't actually afford us that much insight
link |
00:12:43.920
into what healthy social bonding looks like.
link |
00:12:46.520
So today we're going to focus more
link |
00:12:48.320
on the functional biology,
link |
00:12:50.820
dual meaning of the word functional,
link |
00:12:52.240
as opposed to the pathology of social isolation.
link |
00:12:55.920
However, I do want to point out
link |
00:12:58.060
that social isolation starts to deteriorate
link |
00:13:01.720
certain aspects of brain and body pretty quickly,
link |
00:13:05.440
but how quickly depends, again,
link |
00:13:07.600
on how introverted or extroverted somebody is.
link |
00:13:09.960
So if you're somebody who's socially isolated
link |
00:13:12.000
for the holidays or has been socially isolated
link |
00:13:13.920
for a period of time and is craving social contact,
link |
00:13:16.960
that is a healthy craving.
link |
00:13:18.440
And as we'll learn next,
link |
00:13:20.360
the healthy craving for social contact
link |
00:13:22.400
has a very specific brain circuit,
link |
00:13:24.120
has a very specific neurochemical signature
link |
00:13:26.120
associated with it,
link |
00:13:27.240
and has some remarkable features
link |
00:13:29.400
that you can leverage in social contacts of all kinds.
link |
00:13:32.960
I think some of the more important and exciting work
link |
00:13:35.760
on social bonding comes from the laboratory of Kay Tai.
link |
00:13:40.040
Kay is a professor
link |
00:13:41.040
at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies.
link |
00:13:43.220
She's an investigator with the Howard Hughes
link |
00:13:44.880
Medical Institute.
link |
00:13:46.240
And in recent years,
link |
00:13:47.700
I would say in about the last five or six years,
link |
00:13:49.840
her laboratory has made a fundamental discovery
link |
00:13:53.240
as to why we seek out
link |
00:13:55.500
and put so much effort into social bonds.
link |
00:13:58.420
And the key discovery that she made
link |
00:14:01.000
is that much like hunger, much like temperature,
link |
00:14:04.600
much like thirst,
link |
00:14:06.520
we have brain circuits that are devoted
link |
00:14:09.140
to what's called a social homeostasis.
link |
00:14:12.880
Many of you have probably heard about homeostasis before.
link |
00:14:15.320
Homeostasis is the characteristic
link |
00:14:18.320
of various biological circuits and even individual cells
link |
00:14:21.240
to try and maintain a certain level.
link |
00:14:23.480
It's most easily thought of in the context of hunger.
link |
00:14:25.840
If you don't eat for a while,
link |
00:14:26.820
your drive to pursue food and think about food
link |
00:14:29.520
and make food and spend money on food,
link |
00:14:33.160
and indeed to enjoy food goes up.
link |
00:14:36.160
Whereas when you're well-fed,
link |
00:14:37.800
you don't tend to seek out food
link |
00:14:39.720
with as much vigor or as much intensity.
link |
00:14:42.080
You wouldn't invest as much time, effort, money, et cetera.
link |
00:14:44.900
So homeostasis is the aspect of cells, tissues,
link |
00:14:48.280
and organisms to seek some sort of balance,
link |
00:14:50.980
to regulate themselves.
link |
00:14:53.240
In a crude way,
link |
00:14:54.080
you can think about the thermostat on your home
link |
00:14:56.060
as a homeostatic circuit.
link |
00:14:57.680
When the temperature goes up a little bit,
link |
00:14:59.200
it cools things down to maintain a certain temperature.
link |
00:15:01.640
When the room gets cold, it hits a certain level
link |
00:15:05.400
and a sensor detects that, it clicks on,
link |
00:15:07.920
and then the heat goes on
link |
00:15:08.840
to maintain a certain set temperature.
link |
00:15:10.880
So that's a simple way of thinking about homeostasis.
link |
00:15:13.880
Every homeostatic circuit has three components,
link |
00:15:17.080
or at least three.
link |
00:15:18.240
One is a detector,
link |
00:15:19.960
meaning the organism or the thermostat on your wall
link |
00:15:23.360
has to have some way of detecting
link |
00:15:25.120
what's going on in the environment, all right?
link |
00:15:27.840
In the context of social bonding,
link |
00:15:30.120
whether or not you are interacting with others
link |
00:15:32.040
and whether or not those interactions are going well.
link |
00:15:33.860
So that has to be detected, that's the first thing.
link |
00:15:36.200
Then there has to be a control center,
link |
00:15:37.740
that's the second thing.
link |
00:15:39.280
And the control center is the one that makes the adjustments
link |
00:15:42.920
to, in the case of social bonding,
link |
00:15:44.920
to your behavior and to your psychology.
link |
00:15:47.320
So you'll soon learn that there are ways
link |
00:15:49.720
in which the more time that you spend alone,
link |
00:15:52.160
the more motivated you are to seek out
link |
00:15:54.920
the pictures of faces, the interactions with actual people,
link |
00:15:59.100
physical contact, and so forth.
link |
00:16:01.320
Now that might seem obvious to you,
link |
00:16:02.960
but thanks to the work of Keitai and others,
link |
00:16:06.980
it's remarkable to learn
link |
00:16:08.080
that there are specific brain centers
link |
00:16:09.800
that are adjusting our psychology and biology
link |
00:16:11.900
so that we seek out bonds more aggressively,
link |
00:16:14.560
or maybe we don't because we are perfectly sated
link |
00:16:18.320
or satiated with respect to how much contact
link |
00:16:21.320
we've had with other people.
link |
00:16:23.600
Now, the third component of this homeostatic circuit
link |
00:16:26.820
is the effector.
link |
00:16:28.160
The effector is actually what drives
link |
00:16:30.280
the behavioral response.
link |
00:16:32.200
It's what leads you to pick up your social media
link |
00:16:34.640
and start scrolling.
link |
00:16:35.460
It's what leads you to text a friend.
link |
00:16:37.020
It's what leads you to call a friend or make plans
link |
00:16:40.040
and what leads you to follow through on those plans.
link |
00:16:42.640
So again, those three components are a detector,
link |
00:16:45.520
a control center, and an effector.
link |
00:16:48.520
And as you'll soon learn,
link |
00:16:50.000
the neural circuit that controls this social homeostasis
link |
00:16:53.600
actually has a fourth component.
link |
00:16:55.240
And that fourth component is one
link |
00:16:57.280
that places subjective understanding
link |
00:16:59.520
as to why you are doing what you are doing
link |
00:17:01.840
and establishes your place in a hierarchy.
link |
00:17:06.900
Now, I know the word hierarchy
link |
00:17:08.020
can be a little bit of a barbed wire one
link |
00:17:10.500
because people immediately start thinking
link |
00:17:12.120
about boss and subordinate or in couples,
link |
00:17:16.020
a leader and a follower.
link |
00:17:17.600
But when we talk about social hierarchies
link |
00:17:19.960
in the context of human interactions,
link |
00:17:21.780
social hierarchies are very plastic,
link |
00:17:24.920
meaning in one setting, one person can be the leader.
link |
00:17:28.060
In another setting, the other person can be the leader.
link |
00:17:30.420
You probably have groups of friends or family members
link |
00:17:32.720
where you're constantly passing the baton
link |
00:17:34.920
as to who's going to drive, who's going to navigate,
link |
00:17:37.800
who's going to pick the restaurant,
link |
00:17:40.400
who's going to clear the dishes,
link |
00:17:42.140
and who's going to do certain activities and not others.
link |
00:17:44.640
So hierarchies are very dynamic.
link |
00:17:46.880
And as a consequence,
link |
00:17:48.280
social bonding has to be very plastic and very fluid
link |
00:17:52.040
so that you move from one environment to the next,
link |
00:17:54.440
even with the same people,
link |
00:17:55.880
you have to be able to make those adjustments.
link |
00:17:58.120
And in the case of the social homeostasis circuit,
link |
00:18:01.020
those adjustments are made by a particular brain structure.
link |
00:18:04.020
I've talked about in this podcast before,
link |
00:18:05.540
it's called the prefrontal cortex.
link |
00:18:07.140
It is the seat of our higher consciousness, if you will.
link |
00:18:09.980
It's what allows us to play subjective labels on things
link |
00:18:13.040
so we are not strictly input-output, we're not robotic.
link |
00:18:16.800
Meaning if you go to dinner with a friend
link |
00:18:19.400
and they are exceptional at choosing restaurants,
link |
00:18:22.020
well, in the context of the social homeostasis circuit,
link |
00:18:24.880
your prefrontal cortex would allow them
link |
00:18:26.500
to pick the restaurant because basically
link |
00:18:28.600
they are dominant over you in their capacity
link |
00:18:31.160
to pick good restaurants, at least in this example.
link |
00:18:34.020
Whereas as you leave that restaurant
link |
00:18:36.160
and perhaps you are navigating
link |
00:18:37.380
to where to get a drink after dinner
link |
00:18:39.840
or where to walk through the city,
link |
00:18:41.560
perhaps you have the better sense of direction.
link |
00:18:43.480
And so then the social bonding has to be maintained
link |
00:18:47.080
as you switch the hierarchy, okay?
link |
00:18:49.360
So that's the role of that fourth element,
link |
00:18:51.200
the prefrontal cortex.
link |
00:18:52.600
Now, I just briefly want to touch on some of the brain areas
link |
00:18:56.220
that thanks to the work of Keitai and others,
link |
00:18:58.760
we now know underlie the detection, control, and response.
link |
00:19:03.640
Okay, I call them the detector, control center, and effector
link |
00:19:06.600
because inside of that description
link |
00:19:08.400
isn't just a bunch of names of neural structures.
link |
00:19:11.140
There are also hints
link |
00:19:12.280
as to what the underlying neurochemicals are.
link |
00:19:14.920
And by understanding what the neurochemicals are,
link |
00:19:17.440
you can start to think about tools that you can use
link |
00:19:19.740
to form social bonds and maintain social bonds
link |
00:19:22.960
in better, healthier ways.
link |
00:19:24.640
So let's talk about the detector first.
link |
00:19:26.760
Now, keep in mind that you have your senses.
link |
00:19:28.760
You have your vision, you have your hearing,
link |
00:19:30.540
you have touch, you have smell, you have taste.
link |
00:19:33.360
Sensation, as I've talked about many times before
link |
00:19:36.000
in the podcast, but I'll just remind you,
link |
00:19:37.840
sensation is the conversion of physical stimuli
link |
00:19:41.520
in the environment into electrical and chemical signals
link |
00:19:44.480
in your nervous system.
link |
00:19:45.580
The language of the nervous system
link |
00:19:46.720
is electrical and chemical signals.
link |
00:19:48.520
So photons of light are converted
link |
00:19:50.160
to electrical and chemical signals.
link |
00:19:51.520
Pressure on the skin or light touch on the skin
link |
00:19:54.080
is converted into electrical and chemical signals
link |
00:19:56.120
and so on and so forth.
link |
00:19:57.440
So all of that, of course, is flowing into the nervous system
link |
00:20:00.500
but the detector that underlies social homeostasis
link |
00:20:04.640
involves mainly two structures.
link |
00:20:06.920
One is called the ACC, the anterior cingulate cortex,
link |
00:20:09.360
and the other is the BLA, basolateral amygdala.
link |
00:20:12.120
And when you hear the word amygdala,
link |
00:20:13.240
you're probably thinking fear.
link |
00:20:14.900
But today, as you'll see,
link |
00:20:16.420
the amygdala actually has many different subcompartments
link |
00:20:18.800
and components.
link |
00:20:19.920
And there's a reason why the basolateral amygdala,
link |
00:20:22.600
which is associated with certain aspects
link |
00:20:25.160
of aversive behaviors, meaning moving away
link |
00:20:27.600
from certain types of things or interactions,
link |
00:20:29.880
there's a reason why the BLA is such an integral part
link |
00:20:32.760
of the detector system.
link |
00:20:34.640
And that's because just as it's important to form
link |
00:20:37.080
healthy social bonds, it's vitally important
link |
00:20:39.640
to try and avoid unhealthy social bonds.
link |
00:20:42.520
And so the basolateral amygdala is mainly associated
link |
00:20:45.200
with these aversive type responses
link |
00:20:47.160
of just moving away from certain things.
link |
00:20:51.400
The control center in the social homeostasis circuit
link |
00:20:54.220
involves a brain area called the lateral hypothalamus
link |
00:20:56.880
and the periventricular hypothalamus.
link |
00:21:00.120
The lateral hypothalamus and the periventricular hypothalamus
link |
00:21:03.600
contain neurons that are able to access the hormone system
link |
00:21:08.560
in order to influence the release of things like oxytocin,
link |
00:21:11.560
which is a hormone neuropeptide.
link |
00:21:13.680
It's kind of part hormone, part neurotransmitter.
link |
00:21:16.000
It's kind of a hybrid.
link |
00:21:16.840
We're going to talk a lot about oxytocin today.
link |
00:21:19.400
So we've got the ACC and the BLA.
link |
00:21:21.600
These are areas that are mainly involved
link |
00:21:23.100
in moving away from things, although also toward them.
link |
00:21:25.840
That's the detector.
link |
00:21:26.660
Then we've got the control center,
link |
00:21:28.200
which is in the hypothalamus.
link |
00:21:30.040
And then there's a very special and important area
link |
00:21:34.240
associated with social bonding that I want everyone to learn,
link |
00:21:37.420
which is the dorsal raphe nucleus or DRN,
link |
00:21:40.840
dorsal raphe nucleus.
link |
00:21:42.000
The dorsal raphe nucleus is a small collection of neurons
link |
00:21:44.760
in the midbrain, so it's deep in the brain.
link |
00:21:47.820
And most of the time when you hear about raphe,
link |
00:21:50.880
R-A-P-H-E, by the way, raphe nucleus,
link |
00:21:54.700
you're talking about serotonin.
link |
00:21:56.360
Serotonin is a neuromodulator that is often associated
link |
00:21:59.280
with feelings of satiety after eating, warmth,
link |
00:22:03.080
basically satisfaction with things that you already have.
link |
00:22:07.720
However, within this dorsal raphe nucleus,
link |
00:22:10.740
there is a small subset of neurons that release dopamine.
link |
00:22:14.680
Dopamine is a neuromodulator most often associated
link |
00:22:17.120
with movement, craving, motivation, and desire.
link |
00:22:21.000
And the neural circuits that are rich with dopamine
link |
00:22:24.340
are things like the substantia nigra,
link |
00:22:26.040
the mesolimbic dopamine system, the VTA,
link |
00:22:28.840
the nucleus accumbens, et cetera.
link |
00:22:30.120
Those names don't have to mean anything to you.
link |
00:22:32.040
However, this unique population of dopamine neurons
link |
00:22:35.800
in the raphe is truly unique in that it's responsible
link |
00:22:39.600
for mediating what I've been calling social homeostasis.
link |
00:22:44.320
It is the effector or the response
link |
00:22:46.800
that mediates social homeostasis.
link |
00:22:48.820
Now, I haven't told you exactly what social homeostasis is.
link |
00:22:51.920
Social homeostasis, just like hunger,
link |
00:22:54.800
is the process by which when you lack social interaction,
link |
00:22:59.420
you start to crave it.
link |
00:23:01.560
What's very interesting about the fact
link |
00:23:03.440
that there are dopamine neurons in this raphe structure
link |
00:23:06.680
that is the effector for social homeostasis
link |
00:23:10.400
is that what this means is that when you are not interacting
link |
00:23:15.540
with people at a frequency or intensity
link |
00:23:18.660
that is right for you, dopamine is released into the brain.
link |
00:23:23.660
In most popular conversations about dopamine
link |
00:23:26.300
and even in scientific circles,
link |
00:23:28.020
when you hear dopamine release,
link |
00:23:29.400
you think about reward or feeling good
link |
00:23:31.620
because indeed many behaviors
link |
00:23:33.180
and drugs of abuse increase dopamine.
link |
00:23:36.160
That's one of the reasons
link |
00:23:37.000
they have so much addictive potential.
link |
00:23:39.240
However, dopamine is not associated with feeling good.
link |
00:23:42.380
It is actually the neurochemical that's responsible
link |
00:23:45.440
for movement toward things that feel good.
link |
00:23:48.960
So to zoom out and conceptualize what we have here,
link |
00:23:51.820
we have a brain area that is a detector
link |
00:23:54.420
that either will move us toward or away
link |
00:23:56.340
from certain types of experiences or sensations.
link |
00:24:00.840
We have a control center that is going to release
link |
00:24:03.500
certain hormones and neuropeptides into our brain and blood
link |
00:24:06.700
depending on the sorts of interactions
link |
00:24:08.400
that we happen to be having.
link |
00:24:10.220
And we have this response system,
link |
00:24:12.500
which is the dorsal raphe nucleus
link |
00:24:14.460
that contains dopamine neurons.
link |
00:24:15.940
And when we are not interacting with people
link |
00:24:19.220
at the frequency or intensity that we crave,
link |
00:24:22.100
dopamine is released and that dopamine causes us
link |
00:24:25.240
to seek out social interactions of particular kinds.
link |
00:24:28.840
So let's talk about what social homeostasis is
link |
00:24:31.480
and how it plays out.
link |
00:24:32.740
And again, let's use hunger as an example.
link |
00:24:35.380
So let's say you're a person
link |
00:24:37.020
who eats every three or four hours regularly.
link |
00:24:40.140
So on Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday,
link |
00:24:41.900
you're just accustomed to eating every three or four hours.
link |
00:24:44.880
If just suddenly I steal your meal out of the fridge
link |
00:24:49.380
at work, something I would not do,
link |
00:24:50.780
but just for sake of mental experimentation,
link |
00:24:54.760
that would probably cause you to go and seek out food
link |
00:24:57.840
through some other route.
link |
00:24:58.740
You might buy food, you'd probably be upset first,
link |
00:25:00.740
but then you go buy food or replace the food
link |
00:25:02.480
that you were going to eat.
link |
00:25:03.320
You'd be hungry for that food.
link |
00:25:04.320
And indeed there are hormonal type mechanisms
link |
00:25:08.280
and other mechanisms that when we eat regularly
link |
00:25:11.320
and we predict that food is coming in,
link |
00:25:13.040
we actually start secreting insulin,
link |
00:25:14.540
which is for mobilizing blood sugar,
link |
00:25:16.460
there are hormones in the bloodstream
link |
00:25:18.260
that make us hungry on a regular clock-like schedule,
link |
00:25:21.500
and you would seek out more food.
link |
00:25:24.200
Similarly, if you're somebody who is accustomed
link |
00:25:26.940
to a lot of social interaction,
link |
00:25:29.860
and suddenly I take away that social interaction,
link |
00:25:33.520
you would feel kind of let down.
link |
00:25:35.420
You would crave a replacement social interaction.
link |
00:25:39.020
You might be upset that you had a lunch date with a friend,
link |
00:25:41.420
you're used to having lunch with them every Wednesday,
link |
00:25:43.500
and they cancel and you would crave the interaction, okay?
link |
00:25:47.280
This is called a prosocial craving.
link |
00:25:49.500
And indeed, this is what you see in animals and humans.
link |
00:25:52.540
If you, what's called acutely isolate them,
link |
00:25:56.340
which is just a fancy scientific word of saying,
link |
00:25:58.220
deprive them of social interactions in a short-term basis,
link |
00:26:01.900
they start engaging in prosocial behaviors.
link |
00:26:04.260
They start texting other people,
link |
00:26:05.400
they start seeking out social interactions
link |
00:26:08.020
of different kinds, and that makes perfect sense, right?
link |
00:26:11.460
But thought of from a different side,
link |
00:26:13.940
you could also imagine how, well,
link |
00:26:16.400
if you're getting a social interaction
link |
00:26:18.380
with somebody on a daily or weekly basis,
link |
00:26:20.180
and suddenly you remove that interaction,
link |
00:26:22.740
well, then people might not care.
link |
00:26:24.820
They might just think, well,
link |
00:26:25.660
I'll get the interaction tomorrow or the next year
link |
00:26:27.180
or the next day because they're sated,
link |
00:26:28.860
much in the same way that the person who eats very regularly
link |
00:26:31.180
might say, well, I ate four hours ago
link |
00:26:32.740
and I'll eat eight hours later, no big deal.
link |
00:26:34.280
But that's not what happens.
link |
00:26:35.860
There's a prediction that we are going to have
link |
00:26:38.400
certain types of interactions,
link |
00:26:39.900
and when those interactions don't happen,
link |
00:26:42.460
we replace that lack of interaction
link |
00:26:44.540
with a drive and a motivation
link |
00:26:46.260
to seek out social interaction.
link |
00:26:48.620
And that drive and motivation is caused by,
link |
00:26:52.020
or I should say is driven by dopamine release
link |
00:26:54.700
from that dorsal raphe.
link |
00:26:56.500
And so the takeaway is that when we lack social interaction
link |
00:27:00.100
that we expect, we become prosocial.
link |
00:27:03.060
However, if we are chronically socially isolated,
link |
00:27:07.400
meaning we don't have interactions with people
link |
00:27:09.580
for a long time, we become actually more introverted.
link |
00:27:13.520
This is separate from all of the tacky kind and stuff
link |
00:27:15.900
that I talked about earlier
link |
00:27:16.980
or falling into states of chronic stress,
link |
00:27:19.020
but it's well-established now that in humans and in animals,
link |
00:27:23.220
if you don't give them enough social interaction,
link |
00:27:25.860
they actually become antisocial.
link |
00:27:28.060
And so this is actually a little bit like
link |
00:27:29.940
what one might see with long-term fasting, okay?
link |
00:27:33.020
I give the example of eating every four hours.
link |
00:27:34.820
Now let's give the parallel example of somebody
link |
00:27:38.140
who's been fasting perhaps for two or three days.
link |
00:27:41.140
If they are expecting to eat
link |
00:27:42.940
and then the meal doesn't arrive,
link |
00:27:45.460
they are not necessarily going to immediately
link |
00:27:48.500
try and seek out food.
link |
00:27:49.900
And that's a little bit counterintuitive.
link |
00:27:51.160
You would have thought,
link |
00:27:52.000
well, they haven't eaten in a very long time.
link |
00:27:53.180
They're going to be very motivated to seek out food,
link |
00:27:55.460
but no, they are accustomed to fasting.
link |
00:27:57.960
Similarly, the social homeostasis circuit works in a way
link |
00:28:01.780
such that when we don't have social interactions
link |
00:28:04.380
for a very long time,
link |
00:28:05.820
we start to lose our craving for social interactions.
link |
00:28:09.720
Let's look at the social homeostasis circuit
link |
00:28:11.720
through the lens of what's commonly called
link |
00:28:13.660
introversion and extroversion.
link |
00:28:16.420
Now, typically when we hear about introverts,
link |
00:28:18.300
we think about the quiet person at the party
link |
00:28:20.300
or the person that doesn't want to go out at all.
link |
00:28:22.140
And we think about an extrovert
link |
00:28:23.480
as somebody who's really social,
link |
00:28:24.900
the so-called social butterfly,
link |
00:28:26.540
who enjoys social interactions, is really chatty,
link |
00:28:29.260
is kind of life of the party type person.
link |
00:28:31.000
That's the cliche or the kind of pop psychology cliche.
link |
00:28:33.940
But actually in the psychology literature,
link |
00:28:35.920
that's not really the way it holds up.
link |
00:28:38.900
Many people who appear introverted are actually extroverted.
link |
00:28:42.380
The quiet person at a party could be an extrovert,
link |
00:28:46.900
except that they just don't talk very much.
link |
00:28:49.420
The characteristic of an extrovert is somebody
link |
00:28:51.740
that gets energy or feels good from social interactions.
link |
00:28:55.100
They sort of get a lift.
link |
00:28:56.380
And we can predict that that lift occurs
link |
00:28:59.020
because of some release of dopamine
link |
00:29:01.060
within their brain and body.
link |
00:29:02.140
And indeed there's evidence for that.
link |
00:29:03.460
Neuroimaging studies support that.
link |
00:29:05.520
Other forms of neurobiological analysis
link |
00:29:07.580
support that as well.
link |
00:29:09.100
We can also imagine that the person who's talking a lot
link |
00:29:12.300
is somebody who's very extroverted.
link |
00:29:14.500
But oftentimes people who talk a lot for their work
link |
00:29:17.420
or they're somebody who's very social
link |
00:29:19.500
when you interact with them,
link |
00:29:21.100
that person gets back to their car
link |
00:29:22.620
and is absolutely depleted and exhausted by that interaction
link |
00:29:25.580
or all sorts of social interactions.
link |
00:29:28.400
So we really can't predict whether or not somebody
link |
00:29:31.020
is an introvert or an extrovert
link |
00:29:32.620
simply based on their behavior.
link |
00:29:34.060
It's really more of an internal subjective label.
link |
00:29:37.120
However, if we look at introversion and extroversion
link |
00:29:39.540
through this lens of the social homeostatic set point,
link |
00:29:42.940
and we think about dopamine as this molecule
link |
00:29:45.700
that drives motivation to seek out social interactions,
link |
00:29:50.400
what we can reasonably assume is that introverts are people
link |
00:29:54.660
that when they engage in certain forms
link |
00:29:57.080
of social interaction,
link |
00:29:58.840
either the amount of dopamine that's released
link |
00:30:01.920
is greater than it is in an extrovert.
link |
00:30:04.980
That's right, I said greater than it is in an extrovert.
link |
00:30:07.420
And so they actually feel quite motivated
link |
00:30:10.000
but also satisfied by very brief
link |
00:30:12.920
or we could say sort of sparse social interactions.
link |
00:30:17.260
They don't need a lot of social engagement to feel sated.
link |
00:30:20.520
Again, the parallel example will be hunger.
link |
00:30:22.740
This would be somebody who doesn't need to eat much
link |
00:30:25.000
in order to feel satisfied.
link |
00:30:26.900
Whereas the extrovert, we can reasonably assume
link |
00:30:30.540
releases less dopamine
link |
00:30:32.480
in response to an individual social interaction.
link |
00:30:35.100
And so they need much more social interaction
link |
00:30:37.300
in order to feel filled up by that interaction.
link |
00:30:40.500
And indeed, this is supported
link |
00:30:41.580
by the neurobiological imaging studies.
link |
00:30:43.820
So rather than thinking about introverts and extroverts
link |
00:30:46.220
as chatty versus quiet,
link |
00:30:48.980
it's useful to think about people, maybe yourself,
link |
00:30:52.220
maybe other people you know,
link |
00:30:53.820
as how much social interaction they need
link |
00:30:56.340
in order to bring the social homeostasis into balance.
link |
00:30:59.660
Now there's the fourth component
link |
00:31:01.020
of this social homeostasis circuit that I mentioned before
link |
00:31:03.420
and that's the prefrontal cortex.
link |
00:31:05.180
The prefrontal cortex is involved in thinking
link |
00:31:07.240
and planning and action,
link |
00:31:08.620
and has extensive connections with areas of the brain
link |
00:31:11.580
like the hypothalamus,
link |
00:31:13.260
which is responsible for a lot of motivated drives.
link |
00:31:16.660
It also has connections
link |
00:31:18.020
with the various reward centers of the brain
link |
00:31:19.740
and it can act as kind of an accelerator,
link |
00:31:22.480
meaning it can encourage more electrical activity
link |
00:31:25.500
of other brain centers or as a brake on those brain centers.
link |
00:31:29.320
Really good example, it's kind of a trivial one
link |
00:31:31.900
in the context of today's discussion,
link |
00:31:33.360
but it's a concrete one, so I'll use it,
link |
00:31:35.160
would be, I know many people out there use cold showers
link |
00:31:38.680
as a way to stimulate metabolism
link |
00:31:40.180
and build up resilience and this sort of thing.
link |
00:31:42.180
If you get into a very cold shower
link |
00:31:44.140
and you feel as if you want to get out,
link |
00:31:46.740
but you force yourself to stay in,
link |
00:31:49.300
you're forcing yourself to stay in
link |
00:31:50.560
because your prefrontal cortex
link |
00:31:51.880
is placing some subjective label on that experience.
link |
00:31:54.660
Either you're doing it for a certain benefit
link |
00:31:56.660
or you've got a timer and you're using the timer
link |
00:31:59.600
as the regulator of how long you're going to stay in,
link |
00:32:01.700
basically you're overriding reflexes
link |
00:32:03.500
and that's the main function of the prefrontal cortex.
link |
00:32:06.720
But as I mentioned earlier,
link |
00:32:07.620
the prefrontal cortex components
link |
00:32:09.580
that wire into the social homeostasis circuit
link |
00:32:12.180
are responsible for evaluating
link |
00:32:14.140
where you are in a given hierarchy.
link |
00:32:16.380
And that affords you a ton of flexibility
link |
00:32:19.180
in terms of the types of social interactions
link |
00:32:20.980
that you can engage in
link |
00:32:21.900
and whether or not you're going to spend time
link |
00:32:24.380
with certain people or not,
link |
00:32:26.060
whether or not you're going to engage and then disengage.
link |
00:32:28.020
What do I mean by this?
link |
00:32:28.900
Well, let's say you're an extroverted person.
link |
00:32:31.200
You're somebody that likes a lot of social interaction
link |
00:32:33.480
and you get a lot of dopamine release on whole
link |
00:32:36.940
from a lot of social interaction.
link |
00:32:38.420
So maybe one interaction with a teller at the supermarket
link |
00:32:41.340
isn't really going to give you much dopamine,
link |
00:32:43.220
but going to a party will give you more dopamine
link |
00:32:45.660
and so you seek out these larger social interactions.
link |
00:32:48.980
However, you might go to a party
link |
00:32:50.880
where somebody says something or you see somebody there
link |
00:32:52.960
that you'd much prefer not to see
link |
00:32:54.660
and therefore you decide to leave.
link |
00:32:57.480
The deciding to leave is regulated
link |
00:32:59.380
by that prefrontal cortex component.
link |
00:33:01.540
So it's important to understand
link |
00:33:03.340
that just because there's a homeostatic circuit
link |
00:33:05.300
that involves areas like the amygdala and the hypothalamus
link |
00:33:07.940
and these deep brain regions like the dorsal raphe,
link |
00:33:11.740
as a human being,
link |
00:33:12.580
you have flexibility over your social interactions
link |
00:33:14.660
and that flexibility arrives from those prefrontal circuits.
link |
00:33:17.580
So there's a ton of subjective nature to it.
link |
00:33:20.120
There's a lot of context to it.
link |
00:33:21.900
So while there are some predictable elements
link |
00:33:24.620
of these circuits,
link |
00:33:25.580
they are not simply what we would call plug and chug.
link |
00:33:28.200
You have flexibility.
link |
00:33:29.380
You are able to say, you know, I love parties,
link |
00:33:31.340
but I really don't want to go to that party
link |
00:33:32.960
because so-and-so is there.
link |
00:33:34.220
Or I very much don't like going across town in traffic,
link |
00:33:38.060
but I'm going to do it today
link |
00:33:39.300
because a certain collection of people
link |
00:33:41.620
or perhaps a certain individual
link |
00:33:43.820
will be at that particular party.
link |
00:33:45.380
And so the prefrontal cortex again
link |
00:33:47.020
is what allows you that subjective ruling
link |
00:33:49.620
or ruling over what would otherwise just be reflexes.
link |
00:33:52.840
So now I'd like to drill a little bit deeper
link |
00:33:55.060
into this incredible neural structure
link |
00:33:57.680
that is the dorsal raphe nucleus
link |
00:33:59.780
and the small collection of neurons,
link |
00:34:02.140
the dopamine neurons of the dorsal raphe,
link |
00:34:03.980
because while it's a small collection,
link |
00:34:05.300
they are very powerful.
link |
00:34:08.380
Loneliness has been defined by the great psychologists,
link |
00:34:11.780
John Cacioppo, as the distress that results
link |
00:34:15.060
from discrepancies between ideal
link |
00:34:16.820
and perceived social relationships.
link |
00:34:18.580
Let me repeat that.
link |
00:34:19.400
Loneliness is not just being isolated.
link |
00:34:21.860
Loneliness, as he defines it,
link |
00:34:24.720
is the distress that results from discrepancies
link |
00:34:27.300
between ideal and perceived social relationships.
link |
00:34:30.060
It's when we expect things to be one way
link |
00:34:31.980
and they're actually another way.
link |
00:34:33.580
And which way we expect them to be
link |
00:34:35.980
and which way they turn out, again, is highly subjective.
link |
00:34:39.060
What you expect from friendships
link |
00:34:40.900
and what other people expect from friendships
link |
00:34:42.420
could be entirely different,
link |
00:34:43.780
but the circuit that underlies friendship bonding
link |
00:34:46.220
is exactly the same.
link |
00:34:47.660
And it is this dorsal raphe nucleus
link |
00:34:50.000
and the dopamine neurons in that nucleus
link |
00:34:51.720
that underlie the bond that is social friendship
link |
00:34:54.100
and all types of social bonds.
link |
00:34:56.020
There's a key finding in the literature.
link |
00:34:58.340
The title of this paper is
link |
00:34:59.460
Dorsal Raphe Dopamine Neurons
link |
00:35:01.060
Represent the Experience of Social Isolation.
link |
00:35:03.940
This is a paper from Kay Tye's lab.
link |
00:35:05.660
The first author is Matthews,
link |
00:35:07.220
Gillian Matthews, to be specific.
link |
00:35:09.540
What they did is they were able to selectively activate
link |
00:35:12.420
the dopamine neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus.
link |
00:35:15.540
And when they did that,
link |
00:35:16.900
they induced a loneliness-like state.
link |
00:35:19.380
Now, how did they know it was a loneliness-like state?
link |
00:35:21.860
They knew because it motivated
link |
00:35:23.500
the seeking out of social connections.
link |
00:35:26.460
This is the kind of social hunger
link |
00:35:28.680
that I was referring to before.
link |
00:35:30.580
Whereas when the dopamine neurons
link |
00:35:32.420
of the dorsal raphe are inhibited,
link |
00:35:34.300
meaning their activity is quieted,
link |
00:35:36.740
that suppressed a loneliness state.
link |
00:35:40.860
So that's a little counterintuitive, right?
link |
00:35:42.940
It's a group of neurons that when activated
link |
00:35:45.660
makes you feel lonely.
link |
00:35:47.280
And when this brain area is not activated,
link |
00:35:51.060
it suppresses loneliness.
link |
00:35:52.620
But if you think about it,
link |
00:35:53.460
that's exactly the kind of circuit that you would want
link |
00:35:56.180
in order to drive social behavior.
link |
00:35:58.740
When you're feeling lonely, dopamine is released
link |
00:36:01.660
and it causes you to go out and seek social interactions.
link |
00:36:04.620
When this brain area has enough social interactions,
link |
00:36:08.580
that's sort of a figure of speech,
link |
00:36:09.780
brain areas don't have enough social interactions.
link |
00:36:11.960
But when enough social interactions have happened,
link |
00:36:14.460
that the neurons in this brain area
link |
00:36:15.700
shut down their production of dopamine,
link |
00:36:17.700
well, the loneliness state turns off.
link |
00:36:21.100
So what we think of as loneliness,
link |
00:36:23.100
as this big kind of dark cloud
link |
00:36:25.680
or fog in our psychological landscape,
link |
00:36:29.220
boils down to a very small set of neurons
link |
00:36:31.440
releasing a specific neurochemical for motivation.
link |
00:36:34.540
And to me, this really changes the way
link |
00:36:36.360
that we think about loneliness
link |
00:36:37.820
and that we think about social interactions.
link |
00:36:40.740
There's so much subjective landscape to loneliness
link |
00:36:43.820
and to social interactions.
link |
00:36:45.880
But at the end of the day,
link |
00:36:46.820
what it really is is that we are all social animals
link |
00:36:49.660
to some extent or another,
link |
00:36:51.180
and we all crave social interactions
link |
00:36:53.500
to some extent or another,
link |
00:36:54.860
although the extent will vary
link |
00:36:57.220
depending on where you are
link |
00:36:58.460
in the introversion-extroversion continuum,
link |
00:37:00.820
and it is indeed a continuum.
link |
00:37:02.540
Now, the other aspect of the study that was really important
link |
00:37:05.380
gets back to that issue of hierarchy and social rank.
link |
00:37:09.020
What they found is that depending on where you see yourself
link |
00:37:13.380
in the social rank,
link |
00:37:15.260
the dopamine neurons in the raphe
link |
00:37:17.220
will lead to one consequence or another,
link |
00:37:19.880
meaning moving toward social interactions
link |
00:37:22.380
or moving away from them.
link |
00:37:24.140
So the whole system is set up
link |
00:37:26.060
so that you have a ton of flexibility
link |
00:37:27.820
and control over social interactions.
link |
00:37:29.840
So just a couple of key points and actionable takeaways
link |
00:37:32.460
based on the information I've offered up until now.
link |
00:37:36.260
If you think of yourself as an introvert,
link |
00:37:39.620
it's very likely that you get a lot of dopamine
link |
00:37:42.460
from a few or minimal social interactions,
link |
00:37:46.180
whereas if you're an extrovert,
link |
00:37:47.940
contrary to what you might think,
link |
00:37:49.980
social interactions are not going to flood your system
link |
00:37:52.220
with dopamine.
link |
00:37:53.180
They actually are going to lead to less dopamine release
link |
00:37:57.180
than it would for an introvert,
link |
00:37:58.780
and therefore you're going to need
link |
00:37:59.980
a lot more social interactions
link |
00:38:02.020
in order to feel filled up by those interactions.
link |
00:38:04.980
Now, I've been drawing a lot of parallels
link |
00:38:06.500
between this social seeking
link |
00:38:08.820
or avoiding social isolation and hunger,
link |
00:38:12.380
but is that really the case?
link |
00:38:14.220
And could it be that there are actually interactions
link |
00:38:17.440
between the different drives,
link |
00:38:18.740
meaning could social isolation
link |
00:38:21.500
or the desire to seek out social interactions
link |
00:38:24.240
actually relate to the hunger system and vice versa?
link |
00:38:27.260
And indeed the answer is yes.
link |
00:38:29.580
We don't have 50 different homeostatic systems
link |
00:38:33.680
and 50 different neurochemicals
link |
00:38:36.740
to underlie our drive to eat,
link |
00:38:39.780
our drive for romantic interactions,
link |
00:38:42.860
our drive for friendship interactions.
link |
00:38:46.080
We have essentially one, maybe two,
link |
00:38:49.100
and they all funnel into the same dopamine system.
link |
00:38:51.980
And there's a beautiful paper
link |
00:38:53.500
that illustrates some of the crossover
link |
00:38:55.180
between these different homeostatic drives.
link |
00:38:58.540
The title of the paper is
link |
00:38:59.540
acute social isolation evokes midbrain craving responses
link |
00:39:03.080
similar to hunger.
link |
00:39:04.620
This is from Rebecca Saxe's lab at MIT,
link |
00:39:07.460
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
link |
00:39:09.980
Dr. Kay Tai is also an author on this paper.
link |
00:39:12.340
The paper was published in Nature Neuroscience.
link |
00:39:13.940
It's a really terrific paper.
link |
00:39:16.340
Just to briefly summarize what they did,
link |
00:39:18.660
they took people that were categorized
link |
00:39:20.880
as socially connected, healthy human adults.
link |
00:39:23.140
So these are people that are used
link |
00:39:24.220
to pretty frequent social interactions.
link |
00:39:27.380
And they socially isolated them for about 10 hours.
link |
00:39:31.380
And they had no opportunity to access social media,
link |
00:39:35.040
email, fiction reading even,
link |
00:39:38.180
and certainly didn't have the opportunity
link |
00:39:39.740
to interact with people face-to-face.
link |
00:39:41.800
So what this did is it increased social craving,
link |
00:39:44.660
both objectively, the people said
link |
00:39:46.540
that they were now craving social interactions,
link |
00:39:48.600
and then they did brain imaging
link |
00:39:50.020
in response to images of people,
link |
00:39:52.100
people interacting, food, flowers, other types of stimuli.
link |
00:39:56.060
Some of the stimuli or these images,
link |
00:39:58.380
we call them stimuli, but they're images really,
link |
00:40:01.040
had a lot of social engagement going on in them.
link |
00:40:02.980
Others did not.
link |
00:40:03.820
Some had a lot of faces showing, others did not.
link |
00:40:06.420
And as you might suspect,
link |
00:40:08.140
there was activation of many of the brain areas
link |
00:40:11.540
that we've talked about earlier,
link |
00:40:12.700
dorsal raphe nucleus and other brain areas
link |
00:40:14.660
associated with dopaminergic neurons.
link |
00:40:17.400
When the socially isolated people viewed social cues,
link |
00:40:22.700
people interacting faces and so on,
link |
00:40:24.700
and less so for things like flowers.
link |
00:40:27.100
However, they also had increased responses
link |
00:40:30.880
to images of food, which is interesting,
link |
00:40:34.380
and actually is consistent with the literature
link |
00:40:36.820
that when people are socially isolated,
link |
00:40:38.680
they often will start eating more,
link |
00:40:41.140
or they will change the nature of the foods that they eat.
link |
00:40:43.940
Now, we think of that as comfort foods
link |
00:40:46.300
or soothing oneself through eating
link |
00:40:48.900
rather than social interaction as a kind of pathology.
link |
00:40:51.900
But while it might not be healthy,
link |
00:40:53.840
depending on the context and the person,
link |
00:40:56.060
it's really important to understand
link |
00:40:57.420
that the reason that happens
link |
00:40:59.640
is because we have a common circuit
link |
00:41:01.780
and that the system, meaning the person,
link |
00:41:03.880
is actually craving dopamine release.
link |
00:41:05.860
They don't consciously know this,
link |
00:41:07.120
this is all subconsciously carried out,
link |
00:41:08.740
but they're craving dopamine release.
link |
00:41:10.420
And if they can't get it from social interactions
link |
00:41:12.460
as they normally would, they'll start seeking it from food.
link |
00:41:15.500
Now, they did an important reverse experiment as well
link |
00:41:18.900
where they had subjects go on 10 hours of food fasting.
link |
00:41:22.640
Now, these were not people that were familiar with fasting.
link |
00:41:25.520
They weren't doing intermittent fasting.
link |
00:41:28.240
They were eating more typical meal schedules.
link |
00:41:30.820
And so that created increased hunger, et cetera,
link |
00:41:33.700
but it also increased their appetite, if you will,
link |
00:41:37.060
for social interactions.
link |
00:41:38.740
And so the important point here
link |
00:41:40.340
is that there's a common biology,
link |
00:41:42.340
there's a common circuitry
link |
00:41:43.820
that underlies homeostatic craving of things
link |
00:41:46.900
that maintain us as individuals and as a species.
link |
00:41:50.060
And it really places social interactions
link |
00:41:53.240
as right up there in the list of things
link |
00:41:55.820
that we could consider so vital for our survival
link |
00:41:58.540
and for our health.
link |
00:41:59.380
Things like food, water, social interactions
link |
00:42:02.580
are really sit within a top tier amongst each other,
link |
00:42:07.100
and they use the same common circuitry,
link |
00:42:09.180
dorsal raphe dopamine neurons,
link |
00:42:11.100
in addition to other structures
link |
00:42:12.380
in order to create this drive
link |
00:42:14.800
to seek out certain types of stimuli.
link |
00:42:17.180
Now, this is a very reductionist view of social bonding.
link |
00:42:19.900
I realize that.
link |
00:42:21.360
But it's important to realize
link |
00:42:23.060
that while we place all this subjective context,
link |
00:42:25.540
oh, I miss this person,
link |
00:42:26.820
or I really would like to avoid that person,
link |
00:42:30.380
at the end of the day,
link |
00:42:31.220
it really all funnels into a system
link |
00:42:33.820
whereby a single neurochemical
link |
00:42:35.700
is either being released and motivating us
link |
00:42:38.580
to seek out more of a particular type of interaction
link |
00:42:41.220
or is not released,
link |
00:42:42.120
and therefore we are perfectly comfortable
link |
00:42:43.580
staying exactly where we are.
link |
00:42:45.660
As I say this, some of you are probably thinking,
link |
00:42:47.900
oh, that's probably what happens when you fall in love.
link |
00:42:50.220
And indeed, that's the case.
link |
00:42:51.680
When people enter romantic relationships
link |
00:42:54.300
that to them are very satisfying,
link |
00:42:56.320
there's this period that the theory is
link |
00:42:59.340
that it lasts anywhere from six days to six months,
link |
00:43:02.140
although some people report
link |
00:43:03.100
that this feeling can last many, many years, even decades,
link |
00:43:06.260
of just feeling completely filled up and sated
link |
00:43:10.160
by the experience of being with that person,
link |
00:43:12.740
so much so that cravings for food are reduced,
link |
00:43:15.900
cravings for sleep are reduced.
link |
00:43:17.520
Now, there's all sorts of activities
link |
00:43:18.740
and things that go along with new romantic partnerships
link |
00:43:21.140
that take up time that might get in the way
link |
00:43:23.260
of things like sleep or things like food.
link |
00:43:25.600
But the point is that dopamine is the final common pathway
link |
00:43:29.820
by which we seek out things
link |
00:43:31.700
and we end up feeling as if we are satisfied
link |
00:43:34.380
by certain types of interactions.
link |
00:43:36.500
Now, similarly, if you've ever been isolated
link |
00:43:38.620
for a long period of time,
link |
00:43:40.380
your focus might've shifted to what you're going to eat,
link |
00:43:43.620
what you're going to cook for dinner
link |
00:43:44.720
in a much more heightened way,
link |
00:43:46.500
the importance of those sensory stimuli
link |
00:43:48.780
and those types of interactions,
link |
00:43:50.100
and indeed the taste of food itself expands.
link |
00:43:52.820
So normally, when we are in social relationships
link |
00:43:55.700
that are ones that are familiar to us,
link |
00:43:58.120
we have a balance of these different drives.
link |
00:44:00.260
But when one particular drive takes over
link |
00:44:03.300
and we are very focused on it,
link |
00:44:05.820
because they all funnel into the same circuitry,
link |
00:44:08.480
there really isn't the seeking out
link |
00:44:10.440
of certain types of behaviors like food seeking
link |
00:44:12.280
when we're newly in love.
link |
00:44:13.900
Now, that doesn't mean that food won't taste good to us
link |
00:44:16.140
or that we don't seek it.
link |
00:44:16.980
And indeed, there are experiments that have been done
link |
00:44:18.740
where if people have just fallen in love,
link |
00:44:20.260
the taste of a strawberry can just be incredible.
link |
00:44:22.960
The other effect of dopamine is that it changes
link |
00:44:25.020
the way that we interpret sensory stimuli.
link |
00:44:26.860
Our detectors actually change
link |
00:44:28.980
when we are in heightened states
link |
00:44:30.420
of dopaminergic activity or drive.
link |
00:44:33.340
Basically, what this means is that things seem better
link |
00:44:35.700
than they would when we have less dopamine in our system.
link |
00:44:38.900
The point here is that there's a lot of crossover.
link |
00:44:41.460
There's a lot of meshing together
link |
00:44:43.460
of different homeostatic drives,
link |
00:44:45.200
that they don't exist in separate channels.
link |
00:44:47.540
And it's only under conditions
link |
00:44:49.380
in which one particular homeostatic drive
link |
00:44:51.940
is kind of being played out to the extreme,
link |
00:44:53.820
such as the example of falling in love,
link |
00:44:56.180
that we tend to avoid or sort of overlook
link |
00:44:59.900
the other homeostatic drives.
link |
00:45:01.300
And that's because simply we're getting enough dopamine,
link |
00:45:04.020
we don't need anymore.
link |
00:45:05.600
Up until now, I've been focused
link |
00:45:06.820
on the organizational logic of social bonding,
link |
00:45:09.620
which is really just nerd speak
link |
00:45:10.860
for how is it that we form bonds, avoid bonds?
link |
00:45:13.500
Why do people seek out more or fewer bonds
link |
00:45:15.660
than others, et cetera?
link |
00:45:17.260
Now I'd like to shift gears a bit
link |
00:45:19.600
and focus on what are some things that we can do
link |
00:45:23.060
to encourage the formation of healthy bonds.
link |
00:45:27.620
There's a beautiful study that was published
link |
00:45:29.260
in Cell Reports, Cell Press Journal, excellent journal.
link |
00:45:32.420
The title of this paper is
link |
00:45:33.260
is Conscious Processing of Narrative Stimuli
link |
00:45:35.380
Synchronizes Heart Rate Between Individuals.
link |
00:45:38.040
I mentioned this on a previous podcast,
link |
00:45:39.740
but I'd like to mention it again
link |
00:45:41.180
and go into a little bit more depth
link |
00:45:42.340
because it points to specific actionable items
link |
00:45:44.960
that we can all use in order to enhance the quality
link |
00:45:48.000
and depth of social bonds of all kinds.
link |
00:45:51.260
Now, this study involved a very simple type of experiment.
link |
00:45:54.740
They had people listen to a story.
link |
00:45:56.440
Everybody in the study listened to the same story,
link |
00:45:59.100
but they listened to that story at different times
link |
00:46:00.880
and indeed in different locations.
link |
00:46:02.800
So different people, same story.
link |
00:46:05.860
And they measured things like heart rate.
link |
00:46:07.860
They measured breathing, et cetera.
link |
00:46:09.620
Now, what was the motivation for doing this?
link |
00:46:11.780
Well, there's a long standing literature showing
link |
00:46:14.420
that our physiology, things like our heart rate,
link |
00:46:17.180
our breathing, our skin conductance,
link |
00:46:19.000
meaning the amount of sweating,
link |
00:46:22.180
can be synchronized between individuals.
link |
00:46:24.820
And that synchronization can occur
link |
00:46:27.860
according to a variety of different things.
link |
00:46:29.540
There've been studies that have people look at one another
link |
00:46:31.500
and they look and actually see that their pupil size
link |
00:46:34.540
of their eyes starts to synchronize.
link |
00:46:36.780
People's breathing can synchronize.
link |
00:46:38.940
People's body temperatures can even start to synchronize
link |
00:46:42.500
or at least shifts in body temperature can synchronize.
link |
00:46:44.620
One person gets cooler, the other person gets cooler.
link |
00:46:46.820
A lot of this is subconscious.
link |
00:46:48.540
Some of it can be detected by conscious cues
link |
00:46:50.600
like flushing of the skin
link |
00:46:51.660
or actually seeing someone's pupils change.
link |
00:46:53.720
But actually the pupil reflex is a really good example
link |
00:46:56.700
whereby except for rare cases
link |
00:46:59.780
and certain highly trained individuals,
link |
00:47:01.580
most people can't control their pupil reflexes
link |
00:47:03.940
in a very deliberate way.
link |
00:47:05.300
It's truly a reflex.
link |
00:47:06.920
It's an autonomic reflex.
link |
00:47:08.920
So there's a lot of literature showing
link |
00:47:10.420
that within small groups or two people,
link |
00:47:14.060
these physiological signals can be synchronized.
link |
00:47:15.960
What this study found was that when people listen
link |
00:47:19.180
to the same story but at different times,
link |
00:47:21.580
their heart rates start to synchronize.
link |
00:47:23.760
This is incredible because people are listening
link |
00:47:25.380
to the story at different times,
link |
00:47:27.000
but the gaps between their heartbeats
link |
00:47:28.460
become very stereotyped
link |
00:47:29.820
and map almost precisely onto one another.
link |
00:47:32.660
That's incredible.
link |
00:47:34.580
Now, we also know from an extensive literature
link |
00:47:37.820
that the quality and perceived depth of a social bond
link |
00:47:43.180
correlates very strongly
link |
00:47:45.140
with how much physiological synchronization there is
link |
00:47:48.760
between individuals.
link |
00:47:50.580
In other words, when your bodies feel the same,
link |
00:47:54.500
you tend to feel more bonded to somebody else.
link |
00:47:57.620
And so this whole thing is a rather circular argument.
link |
00:48:00.380
When you feel closer to somebody else,
link |
00:48:01.980
your physiology synchronize.
link |
00:48:04.040
And the reverse is true as well.
link |
00:48:06.140
When your physiologies are synchronized,
link |
00:48:07.740
you feel closer to other people.
link |
00:48:09.780
This is what I call the concert phenomenon.
link |
00:48:11.540
If you ever go to see your favorite band
link |
00:48:14.260
or you go to a concert that you particularly love,
link |
00:48:17.080
you will often look over at somebody
link |
00:48:19.140
and you'll see them enjoying the same thing.
link |
00:48:21.040
And they're often in a similar state as you are.
link |
00:48:23.440
Maybe the sort of like favorite song comes on
link |
00:48:25.860
and you actually feel connected to that person.
link |
00:48:28.960
You feel like you're in,
link |
00:48:29.800
obviously there's a shared experience,
link |
00:48:31.820
but there's also a shared physiological response
link |
00:48:34.520
to that experience.
link |
00:48:35.700
And so this can happen en masse with large groups of people,
link |
00:48:39.060
or it can happen just between two individuals.
link |
00:48:41.480
And as this study points out,
link |
00:48:43.200
it can actually happen between individuals
link |
00:48:44.820
without them actually interacting with one another
link |
00:48:46.940
when the story they are listening to is the anchor
link |
00:48:49.820
or the driver of their physiology.
link |
00:48:51.740
This really points to the fact
link |
00:48:53.900
that the body and the brain are reciprocally connected.
link |
00:48:57.100
Yes, indeed, what we think, what we hear, what we feel
link |
00:49:00.740
drives our physiology, our heartbeat,
link |
00:49:02.980
our respiration, et cetera,
link |
00:49:04.500
but our heartbeat and respiration
link |
00:49:06.080
also are influencing our state of mind.
link |
00:49:09.240
And in this case,
link |
00:49:10.080
it's encouraging certain types of social bonds
link |
00:49:12.900
when our heart rates are synchronized.
link |
00:49:15.580
You can leverage this.
link |
00:49:16.940
How can you leverage this?
link |
00:49:17.940
Well, let's take a upcoming example of the holidays.
link |
00:49:21.780
There's a sort of a joke.
link |
00:49:24.280
I think it was Ram Dass, sort of Buddhist philosopher type
link |
00:49:29.020
that said, if you think you're enlightened,
link |
00:49:31.740
go visit your parents.
link |
00:49:32.940
And I think what he was referring to
link |
00:49:34.400
is that some people, not all people,
link |
00:49:36.320
have challenging relationships with their parents.
link |
00:49:38.340
We're going to talk about child-parent attachment
link |
00:49:40.600
and interactions in a few minutes,
link |
00:49:42.080
but some people have a wonderful relationship
link |
00:49:45.340
to both their parents and more power to them.
link |
00:49:47.820
I think that's wonderful.
link |
00:49:49.440
We should all be so lucky.
link |
00:49:50.900
Many people have challenged relationships
link |
00:49:52.620
with their parents,
link |
00:49:53.440
or they have a great relationship with their parents,
link |
00:49:55.700
but their parents know, or they know how to drive that dart
link |
00:49:59.580
right into that particular soft piece of psychological flesh
link |
00:50:03.020
by saying just the slightest thing,
link |
00:50:05.060
or even by raising their eyebrow or rolling their eyes,
link |
00:50:07.620
or the tone in which they do something.
link |
00:50:09.100
This is also true between siblings.
link |
00:50:11.400
I think many of you can think of examples
link |
00:50:13.580
where this is true.
link |
00:50:15.760
Many people, when they interact with others,
link |
00:50:18.540
expect that the mere interaction with the other person
link |
00:50:22.220
is going to create the sense of bonding.
link |
00:50:24.700
And often that is the case.
link |
00:50:26.060
For instance, if people are involved in intimate disclosure,
link |
00:50:29.540
if people enjoy each other's company so much
link |
00:50:33.660
that just the mere sight of somebody evokes great feelings
link |
00:50:36.540
and it's mutual, that often can happen.
link |
00:50:38.860
But in many types of social interactions,
link |
00:50:41.700
it's not the direct interaction with that person
link |
00:50:45.180
that makes us feel close to them,
link |
00:50:46.580
but rather it's shared experience.
link |
00:50:49.180
And shared experience is shared physiology.
link |
00:50:52.580
That's the point I'm trying to make by way of this study
link |
00:50:55.660
about conscious processing of narrative stimuli
link |
00:50:57.820
synchronizes heart rate of different individuals.
link |
00:51:00.380
So for instance, if you have a somewhat challenged
link |
00:51:04.020
or a somewhat, let's call it a slight friction
link |
00:51:07.580
in getting close with somebody,
link |
00:51:10.540
or it can be a challenging interaction,
link |
00:51:12.480
oftentimes it's very useful to focus outward
link |
00:51:15.760
on some other common narrative, a movie.
link |
00:51:18.580
Oftentimes people will watch a game together.
link |
00:51:20.620
Actually, there's a lot of critique
link |
00:51:22.460
that people or families will focus outward too much
link |
00:51:26.120
on external events.
link |
00:51:27.460
But these external events can be observing the grandchild
link |
00:51:30.960
and how wonderful they are,
link |
00:51:31.980
or observing the meal and how wonderful it is.
link |
00:51:34.460
Or as we commonly see in various traditions,
link |
00:51:37.720
there's a story that's repeated each year.
link |
00:51:40.000
Certainly in the upcoming holidays,
link |
00:51:41.420
there's Christmas stories, there are themes and traditions.
link |
00:51:45.220
And those themes and traditions
link |
00:51:46.740
anchor a number of different aspects of our psychology.
link |
00:51:49.800
They're really wonderful.
link |
00:51:50.740
They thread through the ages really
link |
00:51:53.300
and allow us to link our own experiences up
link |
00:51:55.900
with previous generations and experiences.
link |
00:51:59.060
But in addition to that, they synchronize our physiologies.
link |
00:52:02.360
And so sometimes it can be useful
link |
00:52:03.960
rather than expecting others to shift our physiology
link |
00:52:06.960
in the way that we wish,
link |
00:52:08.400
or us shifting their physiologies in the way that we wish,
link |
00:52:11.640
and then expecting some bond to mushroom out of that
link |
00:52:14.700
in some beautiful way,
link |
00:52:16.740
to focus on some external stimulus,
link |
00:52:19.900
to focus on something that will synchronize
link |
00:52:21.780
the physiologies of both people.
link |
00:52:23.060
That can act as a bridge
link |
00:52:24.620
in order to establish social bonds.
link |
00:52:26.460
And this is not a hack or a workaround
link |
00:52:28.820
for making terrible relationships good.
link |
00:52:30.700
This is actually at the seat
link |
00:52:32.300
of what we come away from a social interaction with
link |
00:52:35.400
as feeling, wow, that was a really wonderful time.
link |
00:52:38.200
Often a really wonderful time
link |
00:52:40.100
can be by virtue of the specific things that were said
link |
00:52:42.820
or the specific things that one engaged in.
link |
00:52:45.440
But more often than not,
link |
00:52:46.960
the final common pathway, we should say,
link |
00:52:49.300
of great experiences was a great physiological experience
link |
00:52:53.980
and a shared physiological experience.
link |
00:52:56.980
I have a short anecdote that relates to this.
link |
00:52:58.580
I have an older sibling,
link |
00:52:59.580
and she used to say that when she was in college,
link |
00:53:01.540
the best dates that she ever went on
link |
00:53:03.340
were dates where she was asked to go out
link |
00:53:05.620
and listen to music.
link |
00:53:06.700
She pointed out, however, that oftentimes
link |
00:53:09.160
the guys that would ask her out
link |
00:53:10.240
would take her to jazz clubs.
link |
00:53:11.340
She always had the theory
link |
00:53:12.180
that they would ask her to jazz clubs
link |
00:53:13.620
because at jazz clubs, typically you would sit down
link |
00:53:15.860
and then she had to conclude that they couldn't dance.
link |
00:53:18.380
My sister likes to dance.
link |
00:53:19.420
And so anytime someone actually had the nerve
link |
00:53:21.420
to take her dancing,
link |
00:53:22.260
those turned out to be particularly,
link |
00:53:24.740
let's just say, satisfying dates and relationships.
link |
00:53:27.220
At least they lasted longer.
link |
00:53:28.280
That's all I know about them.
link |
00:53:29.120
That's all I want to know about them.
link |
00:53:30.420
She's my sister after all.
link |
00:53:32.060
But the theory behind whoever was asking her out
link |
00:53:35.860
on these dates was the right one,
link |
00:53:37.260
which is that if you want to bond with somebody,
link |
00:53:39.260
you create a common physiological response
link |
00:53:41.800
through a common and shared experience.
link |
00:53:44.460
And that is often a good entry way
link |
00:53:46.940
into establishing whether or not, it's always a question,
link |
00:53:50.040
whether or not there can be common physiological experience
link |
00:53:52.300
between two individuals.
link |
00:53:54.140
Up until now, we've been talking about social bonding
link |
00:53:56.460
through the lens of neural circuits
link |
00:53:58.060
that are already established.
link |
00:53:59.940
However, early in the episode,
link |
00:54:01.460
I mentioned that these very neural circuits
link |
00:54:03.380
that are responsible for social bonding
link |
00:54:05.100
in adult forms of attachment,
link |
00:54:07.260
be it romantic or friendship or otherwise,
link |
00:54:10.320
are actually established during development.
link |
00:54:13.260
One of the more important and I think exciting areas
link |
00:54:16.420
of early attachment as it relates to adult attachment
link |
00:54:20.460
comes to us from the work of Alan Shore.
link |
00:54:23.460
Alan Shore, spelled A-L-L-A-N, Shore, S-C-H-O-R-E,
link |
00:54:28.940
is a psychoanalyst who also has deep understanding
link |
00:54:31.700
of neurobiology of attachment,
link |
00:54:34.740
both in childhood and in adulthood.
link |
00:54:38.060
And he's focused a lot on differences
link |
00:54:40.940
between right brain and left brain forms of attachment.
link |
00:54:44.820
Now, in a early episode of the Huberman Lab Podcast,
link |
00:54:47.740
I touched into the fact that most of what's discussed
link |
00:54:50.740
in the general public and sort of pop psychology
link |
00:54:53.460
and even in some neurobiology courses
link |
00:54:56.380
about right brain versus left brain
link |
00:54:58.820
and one side of the brain being more emotional
link |
00:55:00.780
and the other side being more rational is completely wrong.
link |
00:55:04.940
Most of what I see out there is actually backwards
link |
00:55:08.360
to the way things actually work.
link |
00:55:10.100
And while there is some what we call lateralization
link |
00:55:13.220
of function, meaning certain brain functions
link |
00:55:16.160
are handled by neurons on one side of the brain
link |
00:55:18.660
or the other, the idea that one side of your brain
link |
00:55:21.740
is emotional and the other side of your brain is rational
link |
00:55:24.220
is just simply not true.
link |
00:55:26.420
However, the work of Alan Shore
link |
00:55:28.620
points to some very concrete neural circuits
link |
00:55:31.380
that do have a lateralization bias,
link |
00:55:34.700
meaning they are more right brain than left brain
link |
00:55:36.780
or more left brain than right brain,
link |
00:55:38.900
that underlies certain forms of attachment
link |
00:55:41.400
between child and parent, in particular, child and mother,
link |
00:55:45.940
and that these right brain isms, if you will,
link |
00:55:49.620
and left brain isms for attachment get played out
link |
00:55:53.880
again and again in our forms of attachment as adults.
link |
00:55:58.460
So I'd like to talk about that work briefly now
link |
00:56:00.780
because I think it really points
link |
00:56:01.980
to a number of important features of how we establish bonds
link |
00:56:05.540
and the different routes to establishing bonds.
link |
00:56:08.540
So within the field of psychoanalysis,
link |
00:56:10.300
there's a longstanding discussion, of course,
link |
00:56:12.300
about the so-called unconscious or subconscious,
link |
00:56:14.820
the things that we are not aware of.
link |
00:56:16.700
And I think there's growing evidence pointing to the fact
link |
00:56:19.140
that at least one major component of the subconscious
link |
00:56:22.620
or the unconscious is the so-called autonomic nervous system.
link |
00:56:25.900
The autonomic nervous system
link |
00:56:27.300
is the portion of our nervous system
link |
00:56:28.900
that controls our reflexive breathing, our heart rate,
link |
00:56:31.380
our skin conductance, meaning our sweating, pupil size.
link |
00:56:35.660
It's the aspect of our nervous system
link |
00:56:38.020
that makes us more alert or more calm.
link |
00:56:40.180
It's the so-called sympathetic, meaning for alertness
link |
00:56:42.820
or parasympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system,
link |
00:56:45.340
parasympathetic for more calming responses.
link |
00:56:48.100
Now, what Dr. Schor's work and the work of others
link |
00:56:52.640
is now showing is that early infant parent,
link |
00:56:57.100
in particular infant mother attachment,
link |
00:56:58.820
involves a coordination or synchronization
link |
00:57:02.700
of these right brain circuits and these left brain circuits
link |
00:57:06.560
as they relate to the autonomic nervous system.
link |
00:57:10.740
How does this play out?
link |
00:57:11.800
Well, it plays out where early on as an infant,
link |
00:57:14.700
when you're born, you're truly helpless,
link |
00:57:16.620
you can't feed yourself, you can't warm yourself,
link |
00:57:18.820
you can't change yourself,
link |
00:57:19.900
and you certainly can't ambulate, walk anywhere
link |
00:57:22.780
to get the things that you need.
link |
00:57:25.260
All of those functions, all of those needs, rather,
link |
00:57:28.940
are met by your primary caretaker.
link |
00:57:31.380
Typically, that's the mother.
link |
00:57:33.480
Fathers, of course, play a role also,
link |
00:57:35.220
but because of breastfeeding or even bottle feeding,
link |
00:57:38.020
typically mothers play a more prominent role.
link |
00:57:40.300
I realize there are exceptions, but that's the general rule.
link |
00:57:44.700
There are now brain imaging studies
link |
00:57:46.840
examining the brains of infants and the brains of mothers
link |
00:57:51.100
as they interact and showing that the physical contact
link |
00:57:54.780
between the two, the breathing of the mother and child,
link |
00:57:58.720
the heart rate of the mother and child,
link |
00:58:00.760
and indeed the pupil size of the mother and child
link |
00:58:02.980
are actually actively getting coordinated.
link |
00:58:05.120
In other words, the mother is regulating
link |
00:58:07.540
the infant's autonomic nervous system primarily,
link |
00:58:10.220
and the infant is also regulating
link |
00:58:11.780
the mother's autonomic nervous system.
link |
00:58:13.540
A small coo from a baby or a cry,
link |
00:58:16.180
which is a stress cry from a baby,
link |
00:58:17.580
will definitely regulate the autonomic nervous system
link |
00:58:19.820
of the mother.
link |
00:58:20.900
This whole right brain system is directly tapped
link |
00:58:26.460
into the so-called oxytocin system,
link |
00:58:28.420
and we'll talk more about oxytocin in a moment.
link |
00:58:30.460
Oxytocin, again, being this peptide hormone
link |
00:58:32.840
that is involved in social bonds of all kinds,
link |
00:58:35.340
but that at least in early childhood
link |
00:58:37.640
is very closely associated with milk letdown
link |
00:58:40.060
and milk production.
link |
00:58:41.540
There's actually a lot of stimulation of oxytocin release
link |
00:58:44.660
in the mother by nursing itself,
link |
00:58:47.020
so physical contact with the nipple
link |
00:58:49.940
and by the contact of skin between baby and mother,
link |
00:58:53.720
and there's specificity there.
link |
00:58:55.060
It's not just any baby that can evoke
link |
00:58:57.900
the most amount of oxytocin release from the mother.
link |
00:59:01.020
Now, however, there are examples where just holding a child
link |
00:59:04.340
will evoke oxytocin release in the non-parent
link |
00:59:06.800
or somebody other than the parent.
link |
00:59:08.540
I think most people experience that.
link |
00:59:10.140
That's the new puppy or new baby phenomenon,
link |
00:59:12.580
because indeed puppies can evoke oxytocin release as well.
link |
00:59:16.340
The point is not that oxytocin is only released
link |
00:59:18.720
in response to the primary relationship
link |
00:59:21.460
or the mother and their child,
link |
00:59:23.400
but rather that the amount of oxytocin scales
link |
00:59:26.580
with how closely related one is to that particular child
link |
00:59:29.780
and vice versa.
link |
00:59:31.240
So there's oxytocin release occurring
link |
00:59:33.160
in both the child and the mother.
link |
00:59:35.040
So this right brain system is an emotional
link |
00:59:38.620
but autonomic system.
link |
00:59:40.420
It is below our subconscious detection.
link |
00:59:44.380
Now, as we get older, there's another system
link |
00:59:49.420
that starts to come into play in parent-child interactions,
link |
00:59:53.720
and this also comes into play in sibling interactions
link |
00:59:56.380
and so forth, and that's the left brain system
link |
00:59:58.460
as described by Alan Shore.
link |
01:00:00.180
Now, again, this isn't about emotion versus rationality.
link |
01:00:03.200
This is about autonomic
link |
01:00:04.580
versus more conscious forms of bonding.
link |
01:00:06.700
So on the left brain circuit side,
link |
01:00:09.820
there is evidence for, based on neuroimaging studies,
link |
01:00:12.540
but also animal studies to support the idea
link |
01:00:15.060
that on the left brain side of things,
link |
01:00:17.540
there is a processing more of narratives
link |
01:00:21.020
that are very concrete, logical narratives, okay?
link |
01:00:23.980
And again, I have to zoom out and just really tamp down
link |
01:00:27.660
the idea that it's not that one side of the brain
link |
01:00:29.700
is emotional and the other side is rational,
link |
01:00:31.900
but rather that these two things are happening in parallel,
link |
01:00:34.460
and that there's a bit of a dominance
link |
01:00:36.860
for the left brain circuitry to be involved
link |
01:00:40.340
in the kinds of bonding that are associated
link |
01:00:42.300
with prediction and reward.
link |
01:00:44.180
So a good example would be reading to a child every night,
link |
01:00:48.140
sitting there and reading.
link |
01:00:49.620
I can recall reading to my niece
link |
01:00:51.100
and seeing her parents read to her,
link |
01:00:53.500
and she had no clue whatsoever with what they were saying,
link |
01:00:56.360
because she, well, at least I don't know,
link |
01:00:58.360
but she certainly couldn't speak,
link |
01:00:59.880
but she liked looking at the pictures,
link |
01:01:01.680
and it was a very predictable sort of interaction.
link |
01:01:04.580
It was, okay, out come the books,
link |
01:01:06.260
it was usually here's the bath, then there's the pajamas,
link |
01:01:09.020
then there's the lights go down, then out comes the book,
link |
01:01:11.460
and then there's the interaction between parent and child,
link |
01:01:13.820
which of course usually also involves physical contact.
link |
01:01:16.920
So it's not like the right brain system
link |
01:01:18.220
and the left brain system are operating separately,
link |
01:01:20.260
they're operating in parallel.
link |
01:01:21.900
But that sort of prediction and reward
link |
01:01:24.020
kids like to be read to
link |
01:01:25.500
is generally mediated by this left brain system.
link |
01:01:27.980
And this carries on as children get older
link |
01:01:30.100
and as parents take on and evolve their parenting roles.
link |
01:01:34.340
It's very apparent that healthy social bonding
link |
01:01:38.940
between children and caretaker
link |
01:01:42.020
relies on the fact that both this right brain system
link |
01:01:44.860
and the left brain system are engaged,
link |
01:01:46.820
that there's a synchronization of autonomic function,
link |
01:01:49.940
meaning a joining together in actual somatic feeling,
link |
01:01:54.260
and that there's a synchronization of experience
link |
01:01:58.400
that's more about some outward or external stimulus,
link |
01:02:01.260
like reading a book or watching a show together,
link |
01:02:03.420
or enjoying some common experience of a meal together.
link |
01:02:07.900
And of course, as children get older,
link |
01:02:09.220
they're able to access more and more
link |
01:02:10.900
cognitively sophisticated things.
link |
01:02:12.620
You can watch a movie with them
link |
01:02:13.820
and they'll make predictions about which characters
link |
01:02:15.540
are going to show up, for instance,
link |
01:02:17.140
or you can take them to a concert
link |
01:02:19.860
and they can appreciate the concert or play in that concert,
link |
01:02:22.620
and they appreciate that they're being appreciated, okay?
link |
01:02:24.940
So there are a million different,
link |
01:02:26.000
there's infinite number of examples here,
link |
01:02:27.900
but the idea is that there are two parallel circuits
link |
01:02:30.780
that are important for establishing bonds,
link |
01:02:33.320
and that this is set up very early on in childhood,
link |
01:02:36.340
and that it's neither emotional nor rational, but both.
link |
01:02:40.420
Now, both of these circuits tap into the circuitry
link |
01:02:43.100
that we talked about earlier,
link |
01:02:44.460
where dopamine is released and molecules like serotonin,
link |
01:02:48.580
which again is a neuromodulator
link |
01:02:50.100
more associated with feelings of warmth,
link |
01:02:52.900
comfort, and satisfaction with our immediate surroundings
link |
01:02:55.760
and possessions rather than seeking of things
link |
01:02:57.980
and motivation and drive to go look for things,
link |
01:03:00.200
as is the case with dopamine.
link |
01:03:01.840
So there's still interactions with those systems,
link |
01:03:04.420
but the work of Alan Shore has stimulated
link |
01:03:07.300
a lot of interest in what are these circuits
link |
01:03:10.620
that underlie these autonomic bonding,
link |
01:03:13.540
this matching of heart rate and breathing,
link |
01:03:17.580
and what are the neural circuits that underlie
link |
01:03:20.020
this bonding or this synchronization of experience
link |
01:03:24.480
on the kind of left brain side.
link |
01:03:26.540
And the reason I find this model so attractive
link |
01:03:28.980
is that it's very clear that healthy child-parent bonds
link |
01:03:32.960
are established not by one or the other
link |
01:03:36.100
of these right brain or left brain systems, but by both.
link |
01:03:39.100
And there isn't enough time to go into it right now,
link |
01:03:42.020
but some of you are probably familiar with this idea
link |
01:03:44.900
of anxious attached versus avoidant attached
link |
01:03:47.780
versus there's a kind of dissociative attached model
link |
01:03:51.300
of infant-parent bonding.
link |
01:03:53.680
Just briefly, what's becoming clear
link |
01:03:56.060
from the neurobiological imaging studies
link |
01:03:58.500
is that as people start to advance into adolescence
link |
01:04:03.180
and adulthood and well into their elderly years,
link |
01:04:07.720
the same circuits that were active
link |
01:04:09.700
and established in childhood are repurposed
link |
01:04:12.700
for other forms of attachment.
link |
01:04:14.820
And that to have truly complete bonds
link |
01:04:18.060
with other individuals,
link |
01:04:18.980
but in particular with romantic partners,
link |
01:04:21.540
it's important that there be both synchronization
link |
01:04:24.020
of physiology and synchronization of these more,
link |
01:04:27.140
I guess we could call them more rational
link |
01:04:29.220
or predictive type circuits.
link |
01:04:31.540
So we can leverage this information.
link |
01:04:34.020
We can start to think about what sorts of bonds to us
link |
01:04:37.240
feel very enriching and very complete.
link |
01:04:39.860
We know that we can have, for instance,
link |
01:04:42.060
an emotional connection with somebody,
link |
01:04:44.020
but we can also have a cognitive connection with somebody.
link |
01:04:46.400
I have many colleagues with whom I have
link |
01:04:48.860
deep intellectual connection and convergence with.
link |
01:04:52.780
I wouldn't say that I have deep emotional connection
link |
01:04:56.020
with most of them, a few of them, yes,
link |
01:04:58.180
but most of them, no.
link |
01:05:00.920
Others in my life, for instance,
link |
01:05:03.140
I have a deep emotional connection to,
link |
01:05:05.820
but not a lot of deep cognitive connection to.
link |
01:05:08.340
A good example would be the connection that I had
link |
01:05:10.180
with my bulldog who unfortunately passed away,
link |
01:05:12.580
but Costello, we had a very close emotional connection.
link |
01:05:17.620
It was based on touch, it was based on our walks,
link |
01:05:19.500
it was based on fun, it was very autonomic.
link |
01:05:22.900
We rarely discussed, if ever, what we were doing.
link |
01:05:25.880
We had a felt relationship
link |
01:05:27.900
as opposed to a cognitive relationship.
link |
01:05:29.980
And while I'm sort of half kidding about that as an example,
link |
01:05:33.640
it's a really good example, it was a very real bond.
link |
01:05:36.300
And in fact, just as a brief anecdote,
link |
01:05:38.220
I can remember when Costello was a puppy
link |
01:05:40.060
and I was entirely responsible for his wellbeing,
link |
01:05:43.240
I, like any parent of any infant,
link |
01:05:45.780
I lost my appetite for those few weeks
link |
01:05:48.460
when I was house training him,
link |
01:05:49.780
and I seemed to lose all ability
link |
01:05:52.240
to process any cognitive information.
link |
01:05:54.060
Now, I was also sleep deprived,
link |
01:05:55.640
but I was entirely focused on the autonomic bond
link |
01:05:59.180
that we were forming.
link |
01:06:00.220
Now, thankfully, that eventually
link |
01:06:01.880
was established pretty quickly.
link |
01:06:03.780
Basically, I went on to just basically feed him,
link |
01:06:06.860
walk him and do everything for him,
link |
01:06:08.480
and we had a wonderful relationship.
link |
01:06:10.740
Now, it's very clear that what we're talking about here
link |
01:06:12.700
is a form of empathy.
link |
01:06:15.280
Empathy is the ability to feel,
link |
01:06:18.220
or at least think we feel what others feel.
link |
01:06:21.580
Because again, as my colleague and the great bio engineer
link |
01:06:24.840
and psychiatrist at Stanford, Carl Deisseroth,
link |
01:06:27.860
has said, and he was a guest on this podcast,
link |
01:06:31.380
we really don't know how other people feel.
link |
01:06:33.440
We just get the sense that perhaps
link |
01:06:34.940
we are feeling the same thing
link |
01:06:36.060
or we're feeling something different
link |
01:06:37.260
and we infer or we project what they might be thinking.
link |
01:06:41.360
Empathy is this sense that we are sensing
link |
01:06:44.700
what other people are sensing, okay?
link |
01:06:46.340
And there's no real way to verify that
link |
01:06:48.060
except if you're measuring physiologies,
link |
01:06:49.700
you could get some insight into that.
link |
01:06:51.620
In the clinical psychology
link |
01:06:52.780
and in the neurobiological literature now,
link |
01:06:55.920
it's understood that there is both emotional empathy,
link |
01:06:59.340
like actually feeling what somebody is feeling,
link |
01:07:01.420
and what is now called cognitive empathy.
link |
01:07:04.700
Cognitive empathy is this idea
link |
01:07:06.220
that we both see and experience something the same way
link |
01:07:09.900
at a mental level.
link |
01:07:11.580
Emotional empathy is this idea that yes,
link |
01:07:14.140
I can feel what you feel at a visceral,
link |
01:07:17.140
somatic or autonomic level.
link |
01:07:19.780
And it's absolutely clear that strong social bonds
link |
01:07:24.340
between children and caretaker
link |
01:07:26.700
involve both emotional empathy,
link |
01:07:28.500
this autonomic function, and cognitive empathy,
link |
01:07:31.220
that there's a mutual understanding
link |
01:07:33.380
of how the other person feels
link |
01:07:35.980
and how the other person thinks
link |
01:07:38.340
in order to be able to make predictions
link |
01:07:39.840
about what they're going to do.
link |
01:07:41.220
It's also very clear based on the emerging literature
link |
01:07:44.100
that romantic relationships,
link |
01:07:45.720
and to some extent friendships,
link |
01:07:47.640
although friendships have been explored a bit less
link |
01:07:50.100
in the literature,
link |
01:07:51.200
that emotional empathy and cognitive empathy
link |
01:07:54.100
are both required in order to establish
link |
01:07:56.660
what we call a trusting social bond.
link |
01:07:58.860
And there's some beautiful experiments done
link |
01:08:00.980
using neuroimaging of two individuals playing a trust game,
link |
01:08:03.980
essentially a game where you're trying to predict
link |
01:08:05.740
the other person's behavior,
link |
01:08:06.840
whether or not they will behave in a trustworthy way.
link |
01:08:09.060
And these experiments tend to use real money,
link |
01:08:12.460
so there's actually something at stake.
link |
01:08:14.060
And you can more or less predict
link |
01:08:15.760
whether or not somebody feels a lot of trust
link |
01:08:18.340
for somebody else and whether or not they believe
link |
01:08:20.420
they will act in a trustworthy manner
link |
01:08:23.020
based on whether or not they have high levels
link |
01:08:25.060
of both cognitive empathy and emotional empathy.
link |
01:08:28.340
So for those of you that are seeking
link |
01:08:29.660
to establish deeper bonds or bonds of any kind,
link |
01:08:33.000
it's important that you think about synchronization
link |
01:08:35.180
of bodily states, we talked about that earlier,
link |
01:08:37.660
and synchronization of cognitive states.
link |
01:08:40.060
Now that doesn't mean you have to agree on everything.
link |
01:08:41.780
In fact, oftentimes people who feel very close
link |
01:08:44.860
to one another cognitively and emotionally
link |
01:08:47.700
argue about all sorts of things
link |
01:08:48.860
and disagree about a lot of things.
link |
01:08:50.220
In fact, we probably know,
link |
01:08:51.880
I certainly know people and couples
link |
01:08:54.060
that seem to bond through arguing,
link |
01:08:56.100
which is an interesting phenotype in itself.
link |
01:08:59.040
But the point isn't that there be total convergence
link |
01:09:01.700
of opinion or stance,
link |
01:09:03.340
but rather that we understand how the other feels
link |
01:09:06.460
and we believe that they understand how we feel,
link |
01:09:09.100
that we understand how the other person thinks,
link |
01:09:11.380
and that they think that we understand how they think.
link |
01:09:15.220
So it's a reciprocal loop between two people
link |
01:09:17.540
that involves this cognition and involves emotion.
link |
01:09:20.540
And it's grounded, as Dr. Schor has pointed out,
link |
01:09:24.020
in our earliest forms of attachment.
link |
01:09:26.260
And that makes perfect sense
link |
01:09:27.820
because the same sorts of circuits
link |
01:09:29.480
that are responsible for social homeostasis,
link |
01:09:32.260
the kind of right brain and left brain circuits
link |
01:09:33.860
that are responsible for infant-mother attachment,
link |
01:09:37.220
and then later for more intellectual
link |
01:09:39.860
or predictive type attachments between child and caregiver
link |
01:09:43.340
are the exact same circuits that we superimpose
link |
01:09:45.900
into all other types of relationships
link |
01:09:47.980
throughout the rest of our life.
link |
01:09:49.800
And I should just mention that for those of you
link |
01:09:52.180
that might be thinking that you had a less than satisfactory
link |
01:09:57.600
infant-caretaker interaction or form of attachment,
link |
01:10:01.640
you are not alone.
link |
01:10:02.720
And in fact, much of the work that Dr. Schor focuses on
link |
01:10:05.740
is about how those early circumstances
link |
01:10:08.420
can be understood and rewired
link |
01:10:10.520
toward the development of healthy adult attachment.
link |
01:10:13.100
And if you want to check out his work,
link |
01:10:15.020
he's actually got a few YouTube videos out there.
link |
01:10:18.220
Again, it's Alan Schor, spelled S-C-H-O-R-E.
link |
01:10:21.340
I'd love to get him as a guest on the podcast.
link |
01:10:23.340
He also has a book, it's called Right Brain Psychotherapy,
link |
01:10:26.260
and it's an excellent book.
link |
01:10:27.400
It's actually pretty accessible,
link |
01:10:28.540
even if you don't have a background
link |
01:10:29.700
in biology or psychology.
link |
01:10:32.360
I found it to be very interesting.
link |
01:10:34.260
There are a lot of excellent references.
link |
01:10:36.060
And again, if you're listening,
link |
01:10:37.500
Dr. Schor or you know Alan Schor,
link |
01:10:38.920
we'd love to get you on the podcast.
link |
01:10:41.020
One of the key themes to understand
link |
01:10:42.780
about biological processes is that they often work
link |
01:10:46.260
on short timescales and longer timescales.
link |
01:10:48.660
And up until now, we've mainly been talking
link |
01:10:50.420
about the stuff that happens on short timescales.
link |
01:10:52.740
So the kind of synchronization of heart rate
link |
01:10:54.660
or activation of a given set of neurons
link |
01:10:56.780
that dumps some dopamine and causes us
link |
01:10:58.420
to seek out more social interaction or less, for instance.
link |
01:11:02.780
But every biological circuit and function needs
link |
01:11:06.100
to have longstanding effects as well.
link |
01:11:08.780
And typically when you're thinking
link |
01:11:09.900
about longstanding effects in the brain and body,
link |
01:11:12.380
you start looking towards the hormone system.
link |
01:11:14.340
It's not always the case, but more often than not,
link |
01:11:16.940
neurotransmitters and neuromodulators are pretty quick,
link |
01:11:19.900
whereas hormones have longer lasting effects.
link |
01:11:22.140
In fact, a lot of hormones can actually travel
link |
01:11:24.180
to the nucleus of a cell and actually change
link |
01:11:26.060
which genes are expressed.
link |
01:11:28.060
So if ever there was a hormone or hormone-like molecule
link |
01:11:32.140
that's associated with social bonding, it's oxytocin.
link |
01:11:35.940
And oxytocin has gotten a ton of interest
link |
01:11:39.140
in the popular press.
link |
01:11:40.420
I don't know why that is, but perhaps it's because
link |
01:11:42.540
of all the incredible things
link |
01:11:43.540
that oxytocin is associated with.
link |
01:11:46.220
And it is indeed a lot of things.
link |
01:11:48.700
So for instance, oxytocin is released in the brain
link |
01:11:52.500
and binds to receptors in different locations in the body.
link |
01:11:55.540
And the moment you hear different locations
link |
01:11:57.180
in the body, receptors, you should think,
link |
01:11:59.940
well, it's going to have lots of different effects.
link |
01:12:01.660
And indeed it does.
link |
01:12:02.860
Oxytocin is involved in orgasm.
link |
01:12:05.820
It's involved in social recognition.
link |
01:12:08.500
That's right.
link |
01:12:09.340
When you see people that you consider your people,
link |
01:12:11.900
your team, your group, your friends, oxytocin is released.
link |
01:12:16.380
Even if you don't come into physical contact with them.
link |
01:12:19.700
Oxytocin is also associated with pair bonding,
link |
01:12:22.180
the feeling that they are your person
link |
01:12:24.820
and that you are their person is a common language
link |
01:12:27.520
people use.
link |
01:12:28.580
It's also associated with honesty.
link |
01:12:31.440
Believe it or not, there are experiments that show
link |
01:12:33.140
that if people receive oxytocin through an inhalation spray,
link |
01:12:37.320
that they will be more honest and forthcoming
link |
01:12:39.300
about certain things.
link |
01:12:40.840
And the oxytocin system and variants in the oxytocin system
link |
01:12:44.740
have also been associated with autism
link |
01:12:46.620
and various autism spectrum disorders.
link |
01:12:48.660
So there's a huge range of behaviors that it's involved in
link |
01:12:51.020
because you have receptors for oxytocin
link |
01:12:53.060
and lots of different brain structures
link |
01:12:54.740
and areas of the body that do different things.
link |
01:12:56.780
However, there's some very consistent effects of oxytocin
link |
01:12:59.580
that are worth just listing off.
link |
01:13:00.980
And then I'm going to talk about two separate pathways
link |
01:13:04.000
by which oxytocin can manifest its effects
link |
01:13:07.320
and how you can actually regulate oxytocin
link |
01:13:09.380
in ways that are interesting and perhaps useful as well.
link |
01:13:13.220
First of all, oxytocin is involved
link |
01:13:15.160
in the milk letdown reflex, lactation.
link |
01:13:18.380
This makes perfect sense.
link |
01:13:19.420
There needs to be a cue by which the suckling on the nipple
link |
01:13:23.620
of the infant causes the release or letdown of milk
link |
01:13:27.060
and milk letdown and lactation is controlled by prolactin,
link |
01:13:29.860
another hormone, but also oxytocin.
link |
01:13:33.180
Oxytocin is also involved in uterine contraction
link |
01:13:36.180
during childbirth.
link |
01:13:38.320
It's involved in cervical dilation
link |
01:13:39.900
to allow the baby to pass out of the birth canal.
link |
01:13:43.520
So it's involved in induction of breastfeeding and of labor,
link |
01:13:47.660
which is remarkable and especially remarkable
link |
01:13:50.180
given that in males, or at least in some male animals
link |
01:13:52.580
and in some male humans, and I do want to say some,
link |
01:13:54.880
and I'll get back to this,
link |
01:13:55.840
it can be involved in the erection response.
link |
01:13:58.140
It can be involved in the orgasm response
link |
01:14:00.100
in both males and females.
link |
01:14:01.400
Although there, there's a very interesting difference.
link |
01:14:03.780
There's a little bit of controversy about this,
link |
01:14:06.500
but it does appear that in females,
link |
01:14:10.500
sexual stimulation and orgasm cause the release of oxytocin,
link |
01:14:15.460
whereas in males, sexual stimulation
link |
01:14:17.580
does not cause the release of oxytocin,
link |
01:14:19.660
but rather a different molecule vasopressin
link |
01:14:22.320
is triggered by sexual stimulation.
link |
01:14:24.920
But orgasm does trigger the release of oxytocin in males,
link |
01:14:27.840
but with a delay of about 30 minutes.
link |
01:14:31.060
Why that is and the specific function of that is not clear,
link |
01:14:34.140
but it does seem that oxytocin is involved
link |
01:14:36.440
in the sexual response in both males and females.
link |
01:14:39.380
The main types of interactions that release oxytocin
link |
01:14:42.460
at high levels are, first of all,
link |
01:14:45.240
that the interaction be between individuals
link |
01:14:47.920
that see each other as very closely associated, right?
link |
01:14:52.600
So a infant and mother are very closely associated,
link |
01:14:55.740
whether or not it's an adopted infant or not.
link |
01:14:57.520
Oftentimes they are in close contact.
link |
01:14:59.020
Oftentimes they are from the very body of the other.
link |
01:15:01.860
And so the amount or the amplitude of oxytocin release
link |
01:15:05.240
tends to scale with how closely associated individuals are.
link |
01:15:08.380
Just the sight of one's baby or smell of one's baby
link |
01:15:11.500
can evoke oxytocin release and vice versa from the mother.
link |
01:15:15.040
Physical contact even more so.
link |
01:15:17.180
In romantic partners, physical contact,
link |
01:15:20.640
even the sight of a picture of a partner
link |
01:15:22.960
can evoke oxytocin release and sexual desire, also trust.
link |
01:15:27.620
So there's this whole collection of psychological
link |
01:15:29.620
and physiological things that are packaged
link |
01:15:31.880
into the oxytocin system.
link |
01:15:34.100
It's not just a one-way system.
link |
01:15:36.560
Now, a lot of people out there have written to me
link |
01:15:38.780
asking about inhalant oxytocin,
link |
01:15:41.320
asking whether or not that can actually increase
link |
01:15:43.720
the depth or rate of pair bonding.
link |
01:15:46.080
And there does seem to be some evidence for that.
link |
01:15:48.380
Now, I think in most places, oxytocin is prescription,
link |
01:15:51.240
although it might be over the counter in others.
link |
01:15:53.740
I don't know, you have to check where you are.
link |
01:15:55.500
As far as I know, you can't just go out
link |
01:15:56.900
and buy oxytocin nasal spray, although you may be able to.
link |
01:15:59.780
Forgive me, I'm naive to that point.
link |
01:16:01.860
But it's interesting to note that some drugs
link |
01:16:05.940
that are being used in clinical trials
link |
01:16:07.640
for things like trauma and are also used
link |
01:16:09.880
in clinical therapeutic settings for increasing bonding,
link |
01:16:12.980
in particular MDMA, also called ecstasy,
link |
01:16:16.360
increase dopamine and serotonin.
link |
01:16:18.800
We know this.
link |
01:16:19.800
Dopamine and serotonin have a vast number of effects
link |
01:16:22.960
throughout the brain and body
link |
01:16:23.880
that I've talked about some of them today
link |
01:16:25.240
in another podcast.
link |
01:16:26.620
But one of the lesser appreciated effects of MDMA
link |
01:16:31.020
is that it causes huge increases,
link |
01:16:33.200
massive increases in the amount of oxytocin
link |
01:16:35.580
that's released into the brain and body.
link |
01:16:38.140
And MDMA-assisted psychotherapy,
link |
01:16:40.380
while still illegal, as far as I know,
link |
01:16:43.080
certainly in the United States,
link |
01:16:44.060
but in most places throughout the world,
link |
01:16:45.780
is being explored in clinical trials,
link |
01:16:47.800
not just for trauma, not just for depression,
link |
01:16:49.880
not just for eating disorders,
link |
01:16:50.980
but also for reestablishing what seem to be fractured
link |
01:16:55.300
or challenged bonds between romantic partners.
link |
01:16:57.940
And while most of the attention has been focused
link |
01:17:00.100
on the dopaminergic and serotonergic aspects
link |
01:17:03.700
of the MDMA response, it's clear to me,
link |
01:17:06.460
based on my read of the literature,
link |
01:17:08.060
that the enormously elevated oxytocin
link |
01:17:10.900
that occurs during the consumption of MDMA
link |
01:17:14.400
is part of the reason why people experience
link |
01:17:17.100
during the MDMA session and post-MDMA session
link |
01:17:20.820
a much greater degree and depth of kinship
link |
01:17:24.620
or feeling of connection with that person.
link |
01:17:26.700
And it's important to point out
link |
01:17:28.380
that that feeling of connection is of the autonomic type
link |
01:17:31.740
that I was referring to earlier,
link |
01:17:33.060
a la Alan Shore's work, that it's not of the,
link |
01:17:36.240
oh, we think about things the exact same way,
link |
01:17:38.220
we agree on everything now,
link |
01:17:39.560
it's more of that their physiologies are synchronized.
link |
01:17:43.260
So much so that even in individuals within a couple
link |
01:17:47.160
where one does a therapeutic session and the other does not,
link |
01:17:50.880
they still both feel quite more bonded to the other.
link |
01:17:54.060
Now, oftentimes in the clinical therapeutic setting,
link |
01:17:56.540
both members of a couple or romantic partnership,
link |
01:17:59.360
whatever that form it may take, are consuming MDMA
link |
01:18:03.820
and then thereby experiencing elevated oxytocin
link |
01:18:08.020
and this enhanced sense of bonding.
link |
01:18:09.580
And again, it's this autonomic bonding,
link |
01:18:11.420
but it's so powerful,
link |
01:18:13.660
meaning the oxytocin response is so powerful
link |
01:18:15.860
that it doesn't even require that both individuals
link |
01:18:18.820
experience this huge inflection in oxytocin
link |
01:18:21.100
and that's because one person's physiology
link |
01:18:23.740
is influencing the other
link |
01:18:24.900
and oxytocin is this kind of bridging signal
link |
01:18:28.380
that occurs in both nervous systems,
link |
01:18:30.100
synchronizes things like heartbeat.
link |
01:18:31.780
Obviously it's associated with touch
link |
01:18:34.120
and so if people are touching or people are engaging
link |
01:18:36.220
in the sorts of behaviors that I mentioned earlier
link |
01:18:37.920
that can increase oxytocin further,
link |
01:18:40.700
that's going to further increase the depth of the bond.
link |
01:18:43.260
But the point here is that there's actually a hormonal glue
link |
01:18:47.120
between individuals, okay?
link |
01:18:48.940
Infant and mother, friends, teammates,
link |
01:18:52.100
romantic partners and so on
link |
01:18:53.780
and that hormonal glue is oxytocin.
link |
01:18:56.820
Now, people vary in the extent to which they feel
link |
01:19:00.580
or have the capacity to feel bonded to anyone.
link |
01:19:04.340
And it is now generally understood
link |
01:19:07.280
that some of that variation
link |
01:19:09.380
might depend on variations in oxytocin receptors
link |
01:19:12.820
or what are called gene polymorphisms for oxytocin.
link |
01:19:16.000
Genes can have a number of different sequences in them.
link |
01:19:18.500
They're nucleotide sequences.
link |
01:19:19.700
We won't go into genetics right now.
link |
01:19:21.140
As and Gs and Cs and Ts in various combinations
link |
01:19:23.780
are what make up the genes.
link |
01:19:24.740
Genes are transcribed into RNA.
link |
01:19:27.780
RNA is translated into proteins that affect cells, okay?
link |
01:19:32.980
The oxytocin gene encodes for oxytocin
link |
01:19:35.780
and variants in that gene change the amount
link |
01:19:38.660
and function of oxytocin.
link |
01:19:41.580
There's a really interesting study
link |
01:19:43.780
published just this last year in a relatively new journal.
link |
01:19:46.640
That journal has a kind of a unusual name.
link |
01:19:48.720
It's Helion, I think it's Helion and not Helion,
link |
01:19:51.360
but Helion, H-E-L-I-Y-O-N.
link |
01:19:53.640
This is a cell press journal.
link |
01:19:55.420
As far as I can tell, it's a very solid journal.
link |
01:19:57.820
Certainly the cell press label is very stringent.
link |
01:20:01.460
And this paper is entitled
link |
01:20:04.060
the relation between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms,
link |
01:20:07.740
which just means changes in genes or variations in genes,
link |
01:20:12.100
adult attachment and Instagram sociability,
link |
01:20:15.300
an exploratory analysis.
link |
01:20:16.660
This is a really wild study, but I like the study.
link |
01:20:19.000
It's very thorough.
link |
01:20:19.840
First author, last name Carollo, C-A-R-O-L-L-O.
link |
01:20:23.940
And what they found was that by analyzing the genetics
link |
01:20:28.660
of different individuals who are on social media
link |
01:20:32.080
and looking at how many people those individuals follow
link |
01:20:36.820
and how many people follow them
link |
01:20:39.700
and what they come up with
link |
01:20:41.100
is a so-called social desirability index.
link |
01:20:44.080
They were able to correlate in a very straightforward way
link |
01:20:47.820
that people that carry certain variants
link |
01:20:50.340
in the oxytocin and oxytocin receptor genes
link |
01:20:53.760
actually seek out more online
link |
01:20:56.260
social Instagram interactions.
link |
01:20:58.860
So some people I know, I won't name their names,
link |
01:21:01.980
only follow anywhere from zero to six accounts.
link |
01:21:05.660
Other people follow thousands of accounts
link |
01:21:08.760
and they take the ratio of how many accounts people follow
link |
01:21:11.760
versus how many followers they have.
link |
01:21:13.700
Arguably not a perfect measure,
link |
01:21:15.260
but a nice one in the sense that you can do this
link |
01:21:17.400
in a completely unbiased way
link |
01:21:19.240
with many, many thousands of subjects.
link |
01:21:21.620
And then they were able to get genomic analysis
link |
01:21:24.380
from a number of these subjects.
link |
01:21:26.020
And it turns out that people who have,
link |
01:21:29.100
let's say higher levels of oxytocin function
link |
01:21:33.040
or potential levels of oxytocin function
link |
01:21:37.020
actively seek out more social interactions on social media.
link |
01:21:40.980
So this I think represents an important first
link |
01:21:43.300
in the area of how social media and data from social media
link |
01:21:46.380
are starting to merge with biological data
link |
01:21:49.060
in terms of predicting how avidly people
link |
01:21:51.860
will seek out social interactions of an online type.
link |
01:21:55.140
And nowadays we hear a lot about how online
link |
01:21:58.260
we are connected, but we're not really, what is it?
link |
01:22:00.940
We're communicating, but we're not connected
link |
01:22:02.700
or the connections aren't real.
link |
01:22:04.180
I think we're going to need to revisit that.
link |
01:22:06.060
While I'm certainly a believer in the idea
link |
01:22:07.960
that face-to-face communication and common interactions
link |
01:22:10.960
with people standing in the same space
link |
01:22:14.100
or playing sports together, enjoying music together,
link |
01:22:16.620
enjoying meals together is vitally important.
link |
01:22:18.900
There's an entire generation or several generations
link |
01:22:22.140
of people that are coming up
link |
01:22:23.980
who much of their social interaction has been online.
link |
01:22:27.340
And if you think about it,
link |
01:22:28.540
all of the things that we've spelled out earlier
link |
01:22:30.980
about common mental narrative,
link |
01:22:33.540
this left brain system, a la Alan Shore,
link |
01:22:35.740
or autonomic bonding or synchronization of heartbeats
link |
01:22:39.900
according to common stories,
link |
01:22:41.220
all that is happening in online social interactions.
link |
01:22:44.460
When a thousand of us look at the exact same Instagram post,
link |
01:22:48.360
yes, we will have a thousand independent responses to that,
link |
01:22:51.940
but chances are many of us have a similar or same response
link |
01:22:54.900
based on the data that we talked about earlier
link |
01:22:56.840
in synchronization of heartbeats.
link |
01:22:58.340
And so we are socially bonded with other people
link |
01:23:01.340
through social media.
link |
01:23:02.660
And it's very apparent that the oxytocin system
link |
01:23:05.500
is playing some role in that.
link |
01:23:07.620
And this, if we zoom out, makes perfect sense,
link |
01:23:10.440
because again, dopamine, serotonin, prolactin, oxytocin,
link |
01:23:15.380
none of these systems were placed in us
link |
01:23:18.620
or are organized within us in order to encourage specific
link |
01:23:23.060
and only specific types of social interactions.
link |
01:23:25.820
The one that we can say is absolutely critical
link |
01:23:28.020
is the child-parent interaction, right?
link |
01:23:30.340
Because children simply can't take care of themselves,
link |
01:23:32.120
they need a caretaker.
link |
01:23:33.180
I should have said caretaker, not parent.
link |
01:23:35.100
But infants, if they're not taking care, will die.
link |
01:23:38.540
But beyond that, we have evolved or come to realize
link |
01:23:42.540
many different types of social interactions.
link |
01:23:44.720
And online interactions nowadays are very, very common.
link |
01:23:48.140
I'm certainly involved in them.
link |
01:23:49.220
I'm guessing you're involved in them as well.
link |
01:23:50.600
We're involved in one right now, for example.
link |
01:23:53.660
The oxytocin system is absolutely threaded through
link |
01:23:56.820
and largely responsible for those types
link |
01:23:59.460
of social bonds as well.
link |
01:24:01.320
And incidentally, oxytocin is the name of the fifth song
link |
01:24:04.220
on Billie Eilish's second album, Happier Than Ever.
link |
01:24:07.100
So we've covered a lot about the biology
link |
01:24:09.160
and indeed the neural circuitry and neurochemistry
link |
01:24:11.420
and neuroendocrinology of social bonding.
link |
01:24:15.240
I want to make sure that I highlight the key features
link |
01:24:17.740
that go into any and all of your social bonds.
link |
01:24:21.140
First of all, all social bonds have the potential
link |
01:24:24.320
to include both what we called emotional empathy
link |
01:24:27.220
and cognitive empathy.
link |
01:24:28.620
And so if you are interested in establishing
link |
01:24:31.060
and deepening social bonds of any kind,
link |
01:24:33.980
it's important that you put some effort
link |
01:24:36.540
toward this thing that we call emotional empathy,
link |
01:24:39.380
which is really about sharing autonomic experience.
link |
01:24:42.820
Now, depending on the relationship,
link |
01:24:43.960
that will take on different contexts.
link |
01:24:45.260
What's appropriate in one type of bond
link |
01:24:47.020
is not going to be appropriate in another type of bond.
link |
01:24:49.340
Physical contact, for instance,
link |
01:24:50.740
is appropriate for certain types of bonds
link |
01:24:52.960
and not for others.
link |
01:24:54.600
Nonetheless, emotional empathy and the synchronization
link |
01:24:58.540
of autonomic function, heart rate, breathing, et cetera,
link |
01:25:02.060
can be best accomplished by paying attention
link |
01:25:03.820
to external events, in particular narrative, story, music,
link |
01:25:07.440
and perhaps sports or other types of experience
link |
01:25:10.040
as an external stimulus to drive synchrony
link |
01:25:12.780
of those internal states.
link |
01:25:15.200
The other aspect of forming deep bonds
link |
01:25:16.860
is cognitive empathy.
link |
01:25:18.940
Again, cognitive empathy is not about agreeing on things
link |
01:25:22.980
or viewing things the exact same way.
link |
01:25:25.020
It's about really gaining understanding
link |
01:25:27.380
of how somebody else thinks about something,
link |
01:25:30.100
really paying attention to that,
link |
01:25:31.440
and then paying attention to how you think about
link |
01:25:35.180
and feel about something.
link |
01:25:36.740
So that's what cognitive empathy is.
link |
01:25:38.300
So emotional and cognitive empathy together
link |
01:25:40.660
are what make up these really robust bonds
link |
01:25:43.460
of various kinds.
link |
01:25:45.060
Now, we also talked about introversion and extroversion,
link |
01:25:48.020
and I'd like to try and dismantle the common misperceptions
link |
01:25:51.540
about introversion and extroversion,
link |
01:25:52.980
because when we look at the neural circuitry,
link |
01:25:54.420
as you recall, introverts are not people
link |
01:25:57.520
that don't like social interaction.
link |
01:25:59.700
It's just that they feel filled up or sated
link |
01:26:03.360
by less social interaction than would be an extrovert.
link |
01:26:07.540
And that's because,
link |
01:26:08.700
at least according to the social homeostasis circuit model,
link |
01:26:11.660
they actually get more dopamine
link |
01:26:13.620
from less social interaction, okay?
link |
01:26:16.060
It's like somebody who's sated by less amount of food, okay?
link |
01:26:19.580
It doesn't mean they don't have the same appetite.
link |
01:26:21.640
It just means that they get more from less.
link |
01:26:24.380
Whereas extroverts get less dopamine release
link |
01:26:28.760
from an equivalent amount of social interaction.
link |
01:26:31.300
And of course, these aren't precise measurements,
link |
01:26:33.180
but on the whole, extroverts need more social interaction,
link |
01:26:36.520
more frequent, more long-lasting, et cetera,
link |
01:26:39.480
in order to achieve that dopamine threshold.
link |
01:26:41.520
Because again, dopamine is driving
link |
01:26:43.800
that craving of social interaction.
link |
01:26:46.420
And once it's met, then people don't feel
link |
01:26:48.500
like they have to seek social interaction as much.
link |
01:26:51.200
So for those of you that feel
link |
01:26:52.780
as if you're an introvert or extrovert,
link |
01:26:54.980
or that know introverts and extroverts,
link |
01:26:57.100
it's not about how verbal people are.
link |
01:26:59.700
It's not about how much they seek out
link |
01:27:01.340
social interactions per se.
link |
01:27:03.420
It's about how much social interaction
link |
01:27:05.260
is enough for the given person.
link |
01:27:08.120
Now, the whole reason for providing this framework,
link |
01:27:10.200
this biological circuitry, et cetera,
link |
01:27:12.820
is not to simply put a reductionist view
link |
01:27:15.760
on things that you already realized and knew,
link |
01:27:18.220
but rather to give you some leverage points
link |
01:27:20.220
to understand how is it that you form social bonds?
link |
01:27:23.340
How is it that you might be challenged
link |
01:27:24.620
in forming certain types of social bonds?
link |
01:27:26.580
And to think about entry points
link |
01:27:28.500
to both establishing and reinforcing social bonds
link |
01:27:31.240
of different kinds.
link |
01:27:32.620
Hopefully, it will also give you insight
link |
01:27:34.540
into why breakups, whether it be between friendships
link |
01:27:37.940
or romantic partners, can be so painful.
link |
01:27:40.620
A breakup of any kind involves both a breaking
link |
01:27:43.180
of that emotional empathy and that cognitive empathy.
link |
01:27:46.340
And indeed, it has a neurobiological
link |
01:27:49.660
and hormonal underpinning, right?
link |
01:27:51.140
We go into some sense a social isolation,
link |
01:27:54.300
even if we're surrounded by other types of people.
link |
01:27:56.780
If one of our major sources of oxytocin
link |
01:27:59.580
or one of our major sources of dopamine
link |
01:28:01.860
suddenly is not around,
link |
01:28:03.740
that is incredibly devastating to a nervous system.
link |
01:28:06.860
And to borrow from the great psychologist
link |
01:28:09.360
and neurobiologist, Lisa Feldman Barrett,
link |
01:28:11.600
who says, you know, we are not just individuals,
link |
01:28:14.200
we are nervous systems influencing other nervous systems,
link |
01:28:17.460
and their nervous systems are influencing us.
link |
01:28:19.340
I think that's the right way to think about it.
link |
01:28:21.400
So it should come as no surprise
link |
01:28:23.380
that breakups of various kinds are very challenging,
link |
01:28:26.400
regardless of what underlied that breakup,
link |
01:28:29.620
whether or not somebody moving or an actual decision
link |
01:28:31.780
of one person to leave the relationship or both, et cetera.
link |
01:28:35.300
On the more positive side, largely biological,
link |
01:28:39.140
but to some extent, psychological view of social bonding
link |
01:28:43.300
will also allow you to orient in this vast landscape
link |
01:28:46.900
that we call social bonds,
link |
01:28:48.660
to understand why it is perhaps
link |
01:28:50.480
that you seek out so many online interactions.
link |
01:28:52.480
Maybe you have the oxytocin polymorphism
link |
01:28:55.880
that causes you to want more,
link |
01:28:57.740
follow more accounts or interact more with people
link |
01:29:00.100
and comment more, respond to comments, who knows?
link |
01:29:04.940
I'm also hoping that it will allow you
link |
01:29:06.460
to get a lens into how you can strengthen the social bonds
link |
01:29:09.720
that you want to strengthen
link |
01:29:11.100
and to establish new social bonds
link |
01:29:12.700
that you want to establish.
link |
01:29:14.620
None of this is meant to manipulate
link |
01:29:16.780
or leverage social bonds that wouldn't otherwise form.
link |
01:29:19.940
To the contrary, it's about identifying
link |
01:29:22.180
what are the specific routes
link |
01:29:24.460
by which social bonds are created
link |
01:29:27.140
and allowing you, I hope, to work with people
link |
01:29:31.780
that you feel challenged in forming social bonds with,
link |
01:29:34.520
or maybe deciding to completely divorce
link |
01:29:36.540
from those social bonds entirely
link |
01:29:38.140
because there's absolutely no hope
link |
01:29:39.340
of ever forming emotional or cognitive empathy.
link |
01:29:41.940
I certainly acknowledge that that could be the case too.
link |
01:29:44.220
So there's both a light and a dark and a gray zone
link |
01:29:46.540
to this entire thing that we call social bonding.
link |
01:29:48.620
What is not graded, but is absolute, as they say,
link |
01:29:52.540
is that social bonds are vitally important
link |
01:29:54.900
to us as a species,
link |
01:29:56.340
whether or not they are at a distance over social media,
link |
01:29:58.820
whether or not they are in close proximity,
link |
01:30:00.340
actual physical contact.
link |
01:30:02.460
Today, what I've really tried to illustrate
link |
01:30:04.000
is that there are a common set of biological,
link |
01:30:06.860
neurochemical, and hormonal underpinnings
link |
01:30:10.140
to what we call social bonding.
link |
01:30:11.900
And so while it is complex and it is subjective,
link |
01:30:15.320
it involves the hierarchies,
link |
01:30:16.660
it involves our previous upbringing,
link |
01:30:19.220
it involves our goals, et cetera,
link |
01:30:22.020
it is not infinitely complex.
link |
01:30:24.260
And in that sense, it is tractable.
link |
01:30:26.820
Hopefully I've offered you some levers or some entry points
link |
01:30:29.780
under which you can both understand
link |
01:30:31.420
and move towards social bonds
link |
01:30:33.100
that would be more satisfying and more gratifying for you.
link |
01:30:36.720
That's certainly one of the goals.
link |
01:30:37.980
The other one is that hopefully if you're a clinician
link |
01:30:40.520
or simply the friend that people go to
link |
01:30:42.440
or the family member that people go to
link |
01:30:43.780
when they are challenged
link |
01:30:45.060
through various challenges and social bonds,
link |
01:30:47.580
that you can start to perhaps pass along
link |
01:30:49.860
some of the information as a way of people understanding
link |
01:30:54.240
what they're going through as they are breaking up,
link |
01:30:56.440
but also as they are falling in love,
link |
01:30:58.200
as they are forming attachments
link |
01:30:59.860
and as they are being challenged with attachments.
link |
01:31:01.780
That's my hope.
link |
01:31:02.920
And especially as you head into the holidays
link |
01:31:05.180
and end of year, but also as it continues into 2022,
link |
01:31:09.880
I would hope that you would take this knowledge
link |
01:31:11.420
and apply it in any of the ways
link |
01:31:12.820
that you feel are meaningful and adaptive for you.
link |
01:31:15.840
If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast,
link |
01:31:18.300
please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
link |
01:31:20.480
That really helps us.
link |
01:31:21.380
In addition, please put comments
link |
01:31:23.660
in the comment section on YouTube if you have them.
link |
01:31:26.140
And if you have suggestions for future podcast guests
link |
01:31:28.960
that you'd like us to host on the Huberman Lab Podcast,
link |
01:31:31.100
please put those in the comment section as well.
link |
01:31:32.940
We do eventually read all the comments.
link |
01:31:36.060
In addition, please subscribe to the Huberman Lab Podcast
link |
01:31:38.520
on Apple and or Spotify.
link |
01:31:40.180
And on Apple, you have the opportunity
link |
01:31:42.100
to leave us up to a five-star review
link |
01:31:43.780
and a comment if you like.
link |
01:31:45.260
Please also check out our sponsors
link |
01:31:46.780
mentioned at the beginning of the podcast.
link |
01:31:48.500
That's perhaps the best way to support this podcast.
link |
01:31:51.060
And we have a Patreon.
link |
01:31:52.200
It's patreon.com slash Andrew Huberman.
link |
01:31:54.740
And there you can support the podcast
link |
01:31:56.480
at any level that you like.
link |
01:31:58.100
We didn't talk about supplements on today's episode
link |
01:32:00.020
of the Huberman Lab Podcast, but on many episodes we do.
link |
01:32:03.600
While supplements aren't necessary for everybody,
link |
01:32:05.620
many people derive tremendous benefit from them
link |
01:32:07.940
for things like enhancing the depth and quality of sleep,
link |
01:32:10.860
for things like focus, immune system, et cetera.
link |
01:32:14.620
If you'd like to see the supplements that I take,
link |
01:32:16.640
you can go to thorne.com slash the letter U slash Huberman.
link |
01:32:20.820
The reason we partnered with Thorne
link |
01:32:22.580
is because Thorne has the highest levels of stringency
link |
01:32:24.820
in terms of the quality of the ingredients
link |
01:32:27.620
that they include in their supplements
link |
01:32:28.820
and the precision of the amounts
link |
01:32:30.500
of the supplements that they include.
link |
01:32:32.020
This is not true for a lot
link |
01:32:33.100
of other supplement brands out there.
link |
01:32:35.580
Thorne is partnered with the Mayo Clinic,
link |
01:32:37.420
with all the major sports teams,
link |
01:32:38.580
so there's tremendous confidence in their stringency.
link |
01:32:41.180
Again, if you go to thorne.com slash the letter U
link |
01:32:44.080
slash Huberman, you can see all the supplements that I take.
link |
01:32:46.300
You can get 20% off any of those supplements,
link |
01:32:48.420
and if you navigate deeper into the Thorne site
link |
01:32:50.540
through that portal,
link |
01:32:51.420
you can also get 20% off any of the other supplements
link |
01:32:54.040
that Thorne makes.
link |
01:32:55.180
If you're not already following the Huberman Lab
link |
01:32:57.020
on Instagram and Twitter, please do so.
link |
01:32:59.620
On Instagram, I regularly teach short snippets
link |
01:33:02.540
about neuroscience and neuroscience-related tools.
link |
01:33:04.640
Some of that information overlaps
link |
01:33:06.280
with what's covered on the podcast.
link |
01:33:07.780
Often, it does not.
link |
01:33:09.540
So check us out at Huberman Lab on Instagram and on Twitter.
link |
01:33:13.320
And last, but certainly not least,
link |
01:33:15.380
thank you for your interest in science.