back to index

The Science of Hearing, Balance & Accelerated Learning | Huberman Lab Podcast #27



link |
00:00:00.000
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
link |
00:00:02.280
where we discuss science and science-based tools
link |
00:00:04.900
for everyday life.
link |
00:00:09.040
I'm Andrew Huberman,
link |
00:00:10.120
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
link |
00:00:12.960
at Stanford School of Medicine.
link |
00:00:15.080
Today, we're going to talk all about hearing and balance
link |
00:00:18.700
and how you can use your ability to hear specific things
link |
00:00:22.200
and your balance system in order to learn anything faster.
link |
00:00:27.000
The auditory system, meaning the hearing system,
link |
00:00:29.520
and your balance system,
link |
00:00:30.680
which is called the vestibular system,
link |
00:00:33.460
interact with all the other systems of the brain and body
link |
00:00:37.000
and used properly can allow you
link |
00:00:39.240
to learn information more quickly,
link |
00:00:41.020
remember that information longer and with more ease,
link |
00:00:45.040
and you can also improve the way you can hear.
link |
00:00:47.800
You can improve your balance.
link |
00:00:49.440
We're going to talk about tools for all of that.
link |
00:00:53.400
This is one area of science where we understand a lot
link |
00:00:56.260
about the cells and the mechanisms in the ear
link |
00:00:59.240
and in the brain and so forth,
link |
00:01:00.840
so we're going to talk about that a little bit,
link |
00:01:02.480
and then we're going to get directly into protocols,
link |
00:01:05.660
meaning tools.
link |
00:01:06.800
We're also going to talk about ways
link |
00:01:09.380
in which the auditory and balance systems suffer.
link |
00:01:11.920
We're going to talk about tinnitus,
link |
00:01:13.140
which is this ringing of the ears that,
link |
00:01:15.920
unfortunately for people that suffer from it,
link |
00:01:18.420
they really suffer.
link |
00:01:19.840
It's very intrusive for them.
link |
00:01:21.540
We're going to talk about some treatments that can work
link |
00:01:24.400
in some circumstances
link |
00:01:25.760
and some of the more recent emerging treatments
link |
00:01:29.040
that I think many people aren't aware of.
link |
00:01:31.640
We're also going to talk about this,
link |
00:01:33.180
what seems like kind of a weird fact,
link |
00:01:34.840
which is that 70% of people, all people,
link |
00:01:38.800
make what are called autoacoustic emissions.
link |
00:01:41.980
Their ears actually make noises.
link |
00:01:43.520
Chances are your ears are making noises right now,
link |
00:01:46.780
but you can't perceive them,
link |
00:01:48.440
and yet those can have an influence on other people
link |
00:01:51.560
and animals in your environment.
link |
00:01:53.800
It's a fascinating aspect to your biology.
link |
00:01:56.240
You're going to learn a lot about how your biology
link |
00:01:59.080
and brain and ears and the so-called inner ear
link |
00:02:01.940
that's associated with balance,
link |
00:02:03.080
you're going to learn a lot about how all those work.
link |
00:02:05.480
You're going to learn a lot of neuroscience.
link |
00:02:07.420
I'll even tell you what type of music to listen to,
link |
00:02:10.000
and if you listen to me,
link |
00:02:10.960
you can leverage that in order to learn faster.
link |
00:02:13.660
Before we begin talking about the science
link |
00:02:15.580
of hearing and balance and tools
link |
00:02:17.600
that leverage hearing and balance for learning faster,
link |
00:02:20.360
I want to provide some information
link |
00:02:22.080
about another way to learn much faster.
link |
00:02:24.880
There's a paper that was published recently.
link |
00:02:27.820
This is a paper that was published in Cell Reports,
link |
00:02:30.620
an excellent journal.
link |
00:02:32.400
It's a peer-reviewed paper from a really excellent group
link |
00:02:36.500
looking at skill learning.
link |
00:02:39.740
Now, previously, I've talked about
link |
00:02:42.340
how in the attempt to learn skills,
link |
00:02:45.300
the vital thing to do is to get lots of repetitions.
link |
00:02:50.580
You've heard of the 10,000 hours thing.
link |
00:02:52.540
You've heard of lots of different strategies
link |
00:02:55.180
for learning faster, 80-20 rule and all that.
link |
00:02:57.980
The bottom line is you need to generate
link |
00:03:00.960
many, many repetitions of something
link |
00:03:03.500
that you're trying to learn,
link |
00:03:04.660
and the errors that you generate
link |
00:03:06.500
are also very important for learning.
link |
00:03:10.080
It also turns out that taking rest
link |
00:03:14.140
within the learning episode is very important.
link |
00:03:16.520
I want to be really clear what I'm referring to here.
link |
00:03:19.380
In earlier episodes,
link |
00:03:20.980
I've discussed how when you're trying to learn something,
link |
00:03:23.180
it's beneficial, it's been shown in scientific studies,
link |
00:03:26.640
that if you take a 20-minute shallow nap
link |
00:03:31.060
or you simply do nothing after a period of learning,
link |
00:03:33.820
that it enhances the rates of learning
link |
00:03:35.880
and the depth of learning,
link |
00:03:37.060
your ability to learn and remember that information.
link |
00:03:40.820
What I'm about to describe are new data
link |
00:03:43.120
that say that you actually should be injecting rest
link |
00:03:46.620
within the learning episode.
link |
00:03:48.540
I'm not talking about going to sleep while learning.
link |
00:03:51.900
This is the way that the study was done.
link |
00:03:54.340
The study involved having people
link |
00:03:57.060
learn sequences of numbers or keys on a piano.
link |
00:04:00.300
So let's use the keys on a piano example.
link |
00:04:03.180
I'm not a musician, but I think I'll get this correct.
link |
00:04:08.360
They asked people to practice a sequence of keys,
link |
00:04:12.220
G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G,
link |
00:04:16.980
and they would practice that either continually
link |
00:04:19.260
for a given amount of time,
link |
00:04:21.020
or they would just do that for 10 seconds.
link |
00:04:23.300
They would play G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G,
link |
00:04:26.820
G-D-F-E-G for 10 seconds,
link |
00:04:28.420
and then they would take a 10-second pause, a rest.
link |
00:04:31.780
They would just take a space or a period of time
link |
00:04:35.460
where they do nothing for 10 seconds.
link |
00:04:37.080
Then they would go back to G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G.
link |
00:04:40.420
So the two conditions essentially
link |
00:04:44.720
were to have people practice continually,
link |
00:04:46.740
lots of repetitions, or to inject or insert
link |
00:04:50.640
these periods of 10 seconds idle time
link |
00:04:53.420
where they're not doing anything,
link |
00:04:54.540
they're not looking at their phone,
link |
00:04:55.860
they're not focusing on anything,
link |
00:04:57.060
they're just letting their mind drift
link |
00:04:59.020
wherever it wants to go,
link |
00:05:00.620
and they are not touching the keys on the keyboard.
link |
00:05:04.260
What they found was that the rates of learning,
link |
00:05:07.460
the skill acquisition and the retention of the skills
link |
00:05:10.300
was significantly faster
link |
00:05:11.980
when they injected these short periods of rest,
link |
00:05:14.400
these 10-second rest periods.
link |
00:05:16.260
And the rates of learning were,
link |
00:05:19.380
when I say significantly faster, were much, much faster.
link |
00:05:22.780
I'll reveal what that was in just a moment.
link |
00:05:25.120
But you might ask, why would this work?
link |
00:05:28.460
Why would it be that injecting these 10-second rest periods
link |
00:05:31.020
would enhance rates of learning?
link |
00:05:32.740
What they called them was micro-offline gains
link |
00:05:35.300
because they're sort of taking their brain offline
link |
00:05:37.180
from the learning task for a moment.
link |
00:05:38.420
Well, it turns out the brain isn't going offline at all.
link |
00:05:42.060
You've probably heard of the hippocampus,
link |
00:05:43.460
the area of the brain involved in memory,
link |
00:05:45.520
and the neocortex, the area of the brain
link |
00:05:47.220
that's involved in processing sensory information.
link |
00:05:50.340
Well, it turns out that during these brief periods of rest,
link |
00:05:53.500
these 10-second rest periods,
link |
00:05:55.600
the hippocampus and the cortex are active in ways
link |
00:06:00.780
such that you get a 20-times repeat of the GDFEG.
link |
00:06:06.580
It's a temporal compression, as they say.
link |
00:06:08.200
So basically, the rehearsal continues while you rest,
link |
00:06:12.460
but at 20 times the speed.
link |
00:06:14.700
So if you were normally getting just, let's just say,
link |
00:06:17.620
five repetitions of GDFEG, GDFEG, GDFEG per 10 seconds,
link |
00:06:23.100
now you multiply that times 20.
link |
00:06:25.080
In the rest periods, you've practiced it 100 times.
link |
00:06:27.720
Your brain has practiced it.
link |
00:06:29.100
We know this because they were doing brain imaging,
link |
00:06:31.100
functional imaging of these people with brain scanners
link |
00:06:33.780
while they were doing this.
link |
00:06:35.360
This is an absolutely staggering effect.
link |
00:06:38.920
And it's one that, believe it or not,
link |
00:06:41.140
has been hypothesized or thought to exist
link |
00:06:43.460
for a very long time.
link |
00:06:48.380
This effect is called the spacing effect,
link |
00:06:50.900
and it was actually first proposed by Ebington in 1885.
link |
00:06:56.940
And since then, it's been demonstrated
link |
00:06:58.820
for a huge number of different what they call domains,
link |
00:07:01.340
in the cognitive domain, so for learning languages,
link |
00:07:04.180
in the physical domain, so for learning skills
link |
00:07:06.240
that involve a motor sequence.
link |
00:07:08.540
It's been demonstrated for a huge number
link |
00:07:10.720
of different categories of learning.
link |
00:07:14.600
If you want to learn all about the spacing effect
link |
00:07:17.060
and the categories of learning that it can impact,
link |
00:07:19.460
there's a wonderful review article.
link |
00:07:21.360
I'll provide a link to it.
link |
00:07:23.160
The title of the review article is
link |
00:07:25.420
Parallels Between Spacing Effects During Behavioral
link |
00:07:28.220
and Cellular Learning.
link |
00:07:29.100
What that review really does is it ties
link |
00:07:31.040
the behavioral learning and the improvement of skill
link |
00:07:33.660
to the underlying changes in neurons
link |
00:07:37.540
that can explain that learning.
link |
00:07:39.500
I should mention that the paper that I'm referring to,
link |
00:07:41.960
the more recent paper that injects these 10-second
link |
00:07:45.040
little micro offline gains rest periods,
link |
00:07:49.140
is the work of the laboratory of Leonard Cohen,
link |
00:07:52.340
not the musician Leonard Cohen.
link |
00:07:54.300
He passed away, he was not a neuroscientist,
link |
00:07:56.340
a wonderful poet and musician, but not a neuroscientist.
link |
00:07:59.780
Again, the paper was published in Cell Reports,
link |
00:08:01.420
and we will provide a link to the full paper as well.
link |
00:08:04.340
So the takeaway is if you're trying to learn something,
link |
00:08:08.220
you need to get those reps in,
link |
00:08:09.640
but one way that you can get 20 times the number of reps in
link |
00:08:12.940
is by injecting these little 10-second periods
link |
00:08:16.040
of doing nothing.
link |
00:08:17.980
Again, during those rest periods,
link |
00:08:19.580
you really don't want to attend to anything else
link |
00:08:21.900
as much as possible.
link |
00:08:22.780
You could close your eyes if you want,
link |
00:08:24.260
or you can just simply wait and then get right back
link |
00:08:26.780
into generating repetitions.
link |
00:08:28.720
I find these papers that Cell Reports and other journals
link |
00:08:31.960
have been publishing recently to be fascinating
link |
00:08:34.180
because they're really helping us understand
link |
00:08:36.120
what are the best protocols for learning anything,
link |
00:08:39.080
and they really leverage the fact
link |
00:08:41.740
that the brain is willing to generate repetitions for us,
link |
00:08:44.420
provided that we give it the rest that it needs.
link |
00:08:47.660
So inject rest throughout the learning period,
link |
00:08:49.760
and if you can, based on the scientific data,
link |
00:08:52.660
you would also want to take a 20-minute nap
link |
00:08:54.860
or a 20-minute decompressed period
link |
00:08:56.820
where you're not doing anything after a period of learning.
link |
00:09:00.080
I think those could both synergize
link |
00:09:02.960
in order to enhance learning even further,
link |
00:09:05.180
although that hasn't been looked at yet.
link |
00:09:07.020
Before we begin talking about hearing and balance,
link |
00:09:09.540
I just want to mention that this podcast is separate
link |
00:09:11.740
from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
link |
00:09:14.260
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
link |
00:09:16.500
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
link |
00:09:19.380
and science-related tools to the general public.
link |
00:09:22.540
In keeping with that theme,
link |
00:09:24.040
I want to thank the sponsors of today's podcast
link |
00:09:27.100
and make it clear that we only work with sponsors
link |
00:09:29.400
whose products we absolutely love
link |
00:09:31.340
and that we think you will benefit from as well.
link |
00:09:34.780
Our first sponsor is Roca.
link |
00:09:36.840
Roca makes sunglasses and eyeglasses
link |
00:09:38.860
that, in my opinion, are the very highest quality available.
link |
00:09:42.660
The company was founded by two All-American swimmers
link |
00:09:44.900
from Stanford, and everything about their eyeglasses
link |
00:09:47.860
and sunglasses were created with performance in mind.
link |
00:09:52.420
These eyeglasses and sunglasses have a number of features
link |
00:09:55.080
that really make them unique.
link |
00:09:56.220
First of all, they're extremely lightweight.
link |
00:09:58.060
The optical clarity of the lenses is spectacular.
link |
00:10:01.540
And for the sunglasses,
link |
00:10:02.940
they have this really great feature,
link |
00:10:04.260
which is as you move in and out of shadows
link |
00:10:07.640
or as across the day, the amount of sunshine might change,
link |
00:10:11.320
you always experience the world as clear and bright.
link |
00:10:14.360
And that can only come from really understanding
link |
00:10:16.840
how the visual system works.
link |
00:10:18.580
The visual system has all these mechanisms
link |
00:10:20.500
for adaptation and habituation.
link |
00:10:22.660
You don't need to know how those things work,
link |
00:10:24.220
but the folks at Roca clearly do
link |
00:10:26.180
because you put these glasses on
link |
00:10:27.940
and you don't even notice that they're on.
link |
00:10:30.020
They also stay on your face even if you get sweaty.
link |
00:10:32.380
They were designed to be used while active,
link |
00:10:34.740
so running and biking, et cetera, or indoors.
link |
00:10:37.260
One thing that I really like
link |
00:10:38.340
about Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses
link |
00:10:40.540
is that the aesthetic is terrific.
link |
00:10:42.340
Even though they were designed for performance,
link |
00:10:44.540
unlike a lot of sunglasses out there
link |
00:10:46.980
that were designed for performance
link |
00:10:48.300
that look kind of ridiculous, kind of space age,
link |
00:10:50.920
Roca eyeglasses and sunglasses, you could wear anywhere.
link |
00:10:54.060
And the aesthetic is really clean
link |
00:10:56.180
and they have a huge number of different styles
link |
00:10:57.860
to select from.
link |
00:10:59.220
If you'd like to try Roca eyeglasses,
link |
00:11:00.920
you can go to Roca, that's R-O-K-A dot com,
link |
00:11:04.340
and enter the code Huberman
link |
00:11:05.900
to save 20% off your first order.
link |
00:11:08.260
That's Roca, R-O-K-A dot com,
link |
00:11:10.420
and enter the code Huberman at checkout.
link |
00:11:12.620
Today's podcast is also brought to us by Inside Tracker.
link |
00:11:15.960
Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform
link |
00:11:18.380
that analyzes data from your blood and DNA
link |
00:11:21.120
to help you better understand your body
link |
00:11:22.720
and reach your health goals.
link |
00:11:24.620
I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done
link |
00:11:27.740
for the simple reason that many of the factors
link |
00:11:30.660
that impact our immediate and long-term health
link |
00:11:33.180
can only be analyzed from a quality blood test.
link |
00:11:35.960
And now with the advent of DNA tests,
link |
00:11:38.220
we can get further insight and information
link |
00:11:40.980
into how our metabolism is working,
link |
00:11:43.740
how our brain is functioning, how our endocrine system,
link |
00:11:47.580
meaning our hormone system is functioning.
link |
00:11:49.900
One of the issues with a lot of companies and programs
link |
00:11:52.180
that involve getting blood and DNA tests, however,
link |
00:11:54.680
is that you get the information back,
link |
00:11:56.020
you don't know what to do with that information.
link |
00:11:57.620
With Inside Tracker, they make all of that very easy.
link |
00:12:00.160
First of all, they can send someone to your home
link |
00:12:01.980
to take the samples if you like,
link |
00:12:03.320
or you can go to a local clinic.
link |
00:12:06.060
You get the information back,
link |
00:12:07.480
and of course you get all the numbers
link |
00:12:08.820
and levels of hormone factors, metabolic factors, et cetera.
link |
00:12:12.460
But the dashboard in Inside Tracker provides directives
link |
00:12:15.780
so that if you want to bring those numbers up
link |
00:12:17.560
or bring them down,
link |
00:12:18.820
or if you want to keep them in the same range,
link |
00:12:20.500
it points to specific regimens related to nutrition,
link |
00:12:24.420
exercise, and other lifestyle factors.
link |
00:12:26.460
So you can really move around those numbers
link |
00:12:28.180
to best suit your health goals and health status.
link |
00:12:31.460
If you want to try Inside Tracker,
link |
00:12:32.960
you can go to insidetracker.com slash Huberman,
link |
00:12:35.980
and you'll get 25% off any of Inside Tracker's plans.
link |
00:12:39.020
Just use the code Huberman at checkout.
link |
00:12:41.260
Today's podcast is also brought to us by Headspace.
link |
00:12:44.140
Headspace is a meditation app
link |
00:12:45.540
that's backed by 25 published studies.
link |
00:12:48.220
And in addition to those,
link |
00:12:49.580
there are hundreds of studies showing
link |
00:12:51.900
that meditation is beneficial for our brain
link |
00:12:54.860
and for our body.
link |
00:12:56.340
One of the challenges, however, is maintaining
link |
00:12:58.740
a meditation practice.
link |
00:13:00.260
I started meditating a long time ago,
link |
00:13:01.960
but I found it very hard to keep that practice going.
link |
00:13:05.040
Then I discovered the Headspace meditation app,
link |
00:13:07.300
and what I found was that because they have meditations
link |
00:13:09.900
that are very short, as well as some that are longer
link |
00:13:12.080
and some that are much longer,
link |
00:13:14.020
I could maintain my meditation practice.
link |
00:13:15.620
Sometimes I do a short five-minute meditation.
link |
00:13:17.620
Sometimes I do 20-minute meditation.
link |
00:13:19.700
I try and meditate at least 20 minutes per day,
link |
00:13:22.740
but sometimes, some weeks, I only do it five times a week,
link |
00:13:25.540
and I'll just meditate for longer.
link |
00:13:27.140
So with Headspace, you have the full palette
link |
00:13:29.100
of meditations to select from.
link |
00:13:31.120
If you want to try Headspace,
link |
00:13:32.400
you can go to headspace.com slash special offer,
link |
00:13:35.600
and if you do that, you'll get a free one-month trial,
link |
00:13:38.360
so no cost whatsoever,
link |
00:13:40.220
with their full library of meditations.
link |
00:13:41.940
That's the best offer that they have.
link |
00:13:44.020
So again, if you want to try Headspace
link |
00:13:46.200
and you want to get access to all their meditations for free,
link |
00:13:49.280
go to headspace.com slash special offer.
link |
00:13:52.840
Can you hear me?
link |
00:13:53.680
Can you hear me?
link |
00:13:55.960
Okay, well, if you can hear me, that's amazing,
link |
00:13:58.360
because what it means is that my voice
link |
00:14:01.960
is causing little tiny changes in the airwaves,
link |
00:14:04.920
wherever you happen to be,
link |
00:14:06.720
and that your ears and whatever's contained in those ears
link |
00:14:10.480
and in your brain can take those sound waves
link |
00:14:13.700
and make sense of them,
link |
00:14:14.860
and that is an absolutely fantastic
link |
00:14:17.500
and staggering feat of biology,
link |
00:14:20.140
and yet we understand a lot about how that process works.
link |
00:14:23.700
So I'm going to teach it to you now in simple terms
link |
00:14:26.140
over the next few minutes.
link |
00:14:28.100
So what we call ears have a technical name.
link |
00:14:31.820
That technical name is oracles,
link |
00:14:34.660
but more often they're called pinna.
link |
00:14:36.940
The pinna is P-I-N-N-A, pinna,
link |
00:14:40.360
and the pinna of your ears,
link |
00:14:42.980
this outer part that is made of cartilage and stuff,
link |
00:14:45.880
is a range such that it can capture sound
link |
00:14:50.140
in the best way for your head size.
link |
00:14:53.320
We're going to talk about ear size also,
link |
00:14:54.980
because it turns out that your ears change size
link |
00:14:57.160
across the lifespan and that how big your ears are,
link |
00:15:01.020
or rather how fast your ears are changing size
link |
00:15:05.120
is a pretty good indication of how fast you're aging.
link |
00:15:08.140
So we'll get to that in a few minutes,
link |
00:15:09.460
but I want to talk about these things that we call ears
link |
00:15:12.820
and some of the stuff contained within them
link |
00:15:14.940
that allow us to hear.
link |
00:15:16.640
So the shape of these ears that we have
link |
00:15:20.500
is such that it amplifies high frequency sounds.
link |
00:15:23.300
High frequency sounds as the name suggests
link |
00:15:25.500
is the squeakier stuff, right?
link |
00:15:27.020
So low frequency sound.
link |
00:15:28.780
Costello snoring in the background,
link |
00:15:30.080
that's a low frequency sound or high frequency sound, okay?
link |
00:15:34.780
So we have low frequency sounds and high frequency sounds
link |
00:15:37.220
and everything in between.
link |
00:15:38.860
Now those sound waves get captured by our ears
link |
00:15:41.860
and those sound waves,
link |
00:15:43.700
for those of you that don't
link |
00:15:45.060
maybe fully conceptualize sound waves
link |
00:15:47.580
are literally just fluctuations or shifts
link |
00:15:51.960
in the way that air is moving
link |
00:15:55.460
toward your ear and through space.
link |
00:15:57.300
In the same way that water can have waves,
link |
00:15:59.700
the air can have waves, okay?
link |
00:16:01.820
So it's reverberation of air.
link |
00:16:06.060
Those come in through your ears
link |
00:16:08.260
and you have what's called your eardrum.
link |
00:16:10.800
And on the inside of your eardrum,
link |
00:16:13.260
there's a little bony thing
link |
00:16:14.880
that's shaped like a little hammer.
link |
00:16:16.440
So attached to that eardrum,
link |
00:16:18.520
which can move back and forth like a drum,
link |
00:16:20.680
it's like a little membrane,
link |
00:16:22.480
you've got this hammer attached to it.
link |
00:16:24.400
And that hammer has three parts.
link |
00:16:26.300
For those of you that want to know,
link |
00:16:27.300
those three parts are called malleus, incus, and stapes.
link |
00:16:31.500
But basically you can just think about it as a hammer.
link |
00:16:33.500
So you've got this eardrum and then a hammer,
link |
00:16:36.300
and then that hammer has to hammer on something.
link |
00:16:39.060
And what it does is it hammers on a little coiled piece
link |
00:16:43.460
of tissue that we call the cochlea,
link |
00:16:45.700
sometimes called the cochlea,
link |
00:16:47.020
depending on where somebody lives in the country.
link |
00:16:50.020
So typically in the Midwest, on the East Coast,
link |
00:16:51.900
they call them cochlea.
link |
00:16:52.980
And on the West Coast, we call them cochlea.
link |
00:16:54.700
Same thing, okay?
link |
00:16:57.280
So the snail-shaped structure in your inner ear
link |
00:17:01.240
is where sound gets converted into electrical signals
link |
00:17:05.020
that the brain can understand.
link |
00:17:06.860
But I want to just bring your attention to that little hammer
link |
00:17:09.980
because that little hammer is really, really cool.
link |
00:17:12.200
What it means is that sound waves come in through your ears.
link |
00:17:14.420
That's what's happening right now.
link |
00:17:16.260
That eardrum that you have, it's like the top of a drum.
link |
00:17:20.100
It's like a membrane, or it can move back and forth.
link |
00:17:22.980
It's not super rigid.
link |
00:17:24.380
And it moves that little hammer,
link |
00:17:25.820
and then the hammer goes dung, dung, dung, dung, dung,
link |
00:17:28.100
and hits this coil-shaped thing
link |
00:17:31.060
that we're calling the cochlea, okay?
link |
00:17:34.020
Now, the cochlea at one end is more rigid than the other.
link |
00:17:39.620
So one part can move really easily,
link |
00:17:41.460
and the other part doesn't move very easily.
link |
00:17:44.300
And that turns out to be very important
link |
00:17:46.580
for decoding or separating sounds that are low frequency,
link |
00:17:51.900
like Costello's snoring,
link |
00:17:53.700
and sounds that are of high frequency,
link |
00:17:56.300
like a shriek or a shrill.
link |
00:17:59.020
And that's because within that little coiled thing
link |
00:18:03.820
we call the cochlea,
link |
00:18:05.460
you have all these tiny little,
link |
00:18:07.580
what are called hair cells.
link |
00:18:09.740
Now, they look like hairs,
link |
00:18:11.140
but they're not at all related to the hairs on your head
link |
00:18:13.580
or elsewhere on your body.
link |
00:18:15.180
They're just shaped like hairs, so we call them hair cells.
link |
00:18:18.660
Those hair cells, if they move,
link |
00:18:22.100
send signals into the brain
link |
00:18:23.980
that a particular sound is in our environment.
link |
00:18:26.740
And if those hair cells don't move,
link |
00:18:28.620
it means that particular sound is not in our environment,
link |
00:18:32.320
okay?
link |
00:18:33.160
So just to give you the mental picture of this,
link |
00:18:35.600
sound waves are coming in
link |
00:18:36.860
because there's stuff out there making noises like my voice.
link |
00:18:40.940
It's changing the patterns of air around you
link |
00:18:43.980
in very, very subtle ways.
link |
00:18:45.940
That information is getting funneled into your ears
link |
00:18:48.940
because your pinna's are shaped in a particular way.
link |
00:18:51.620
The eardrum then moves this little hammer
link |
00:18:54.140
and the hammer bangs on this little snail-shaped thing.
link |
00:18:57.740
And because that snail-shaped thing at one end
link |
00:19:00.980
is very rigid, it doesn't want to move.
link |
00:19:02.780
And at the other end, it's very flexible.
link |
00:19:05.140
It can separate out high-frequency and low-frequency sounds.
link |
00:19:09.660
And the fact that this thing in your inner ear
link |
00:19:12.260
that we call the cochlea is coiled
link |
00:19:14.440
is actually really important to understand
link |
00:19:17.560
because along its length,
link |
00:19:19.660
it varies in how rigid or flexible it is.
link |
00:19:22.260
I already mentioned that before.
link |
00:19:24.020
And at the base, it's very rigid.
link |
00:19:28.000
And that's where the hair cells, if they move,
link |
00:19:31.420
will make high-frequency sounds.
link |
00:19:33.860
And at the top, what's called the apex,
link |
00:19:35.940
it's very flexible and it's more like a bass drum.
link |
00:19:38.980
So basically what happens is sound waves come into your ears
link |
00:19:42.460
and then at one end of this thing that we call the cochlea,
link |
00:19:46.680
at the top, it's essentially encoding
link |
00:19:50.500
or only responding to sounds that are like,
link |
00:19:52.900
doom, doom, doom, doom.
link |
00:19:55.060
Whereas at the bottom, it responds to high-frequency sounds
link |
00:19:59.060
like a cymbal, tss, tss, tss, tss, tss, tss.
link |
00:20:02.740
Okay, and everywhere in between,
link |
00:20:05.060
we have other frequencies, medium frequencies.
link |
00:20:08.640
Now, this should stagger your mind.
link |
00:20:11.660
If it doesn't already, it should.
link |
00:20:13.720
Because what this means is that
link |
00:20:16.380
everything that's happening around us,
link |
00:20:18.100
whether or not it's music or voices or crying or screaming
link |
00:20:21.460
or screaming of delight from small children
link |
00:20:24.700
who are excited because they're playing
link |
00:20:26.160
or because they get cake,
link |
00:20:28.020
all of that is being broken down into its component parts.
link |
00:20:32.340
And then your brain is making sense of what it means.
link |
00:20:36.100
These things that I've been talking about,
link |
00:20:37.940
like the pinna of your ears and this little hammer
link |
00:20:40.700
and the cochlea, that's all purely mechanical.
link |
00:20:43.740
It has no mind of its own.
link |
00:20:45.440
It's just breaking things down into high frequencies,
link |
00:20:48.060
medium frequencies, and low frequencies.
link |
00:20:50.060
And if you don't understand sound frequency,
link |
00:20:51.860
it's really simple to understand.
link |
00:20:54.060
Just imagine ripples on a pond.
link |
00:20:56.180
And if those ripples are very close together,
link |
00:20:58.780
that's high frequency.
link |
00:20:59.960
They occur at high frequency.
link |
00:21:01.940
If those ripples are further apart, it's low frequency.
link |
00:21:05.360
And obviously, medium frequency is in between.
link |
00:21:07.380
So just like you can have waves in water,
link |
00:21:08.820
you can have waves in air.
link |
00:21:10.140
So that's really how it works.
link |
00:21:12.300
Now, we are all familiar with light
link |
00:21:16.700
and how if you take a prism and put it in front of light,
link |
00:21:20.260
it will split that light into its different wavelengths,
link |
00:21:22.980
its different colors, red, green, blue, et cetera, right?
link |
00:21:26.260
So like the Pink Floyd, Dark Side of the Moon album,
link |
00:21:29.780
I think has a prism and it's converting white light
link |
00:21:32.820
into all the colors,
link |
00:21:34.900
all the wavelengths that are contained in white light.
link |
00:21:37.580
Your cochlea essentially acts as a prism.
link |
00:21:40.740
It takes all the sound in your environment
link |
00:21:42.980
and it splits up those sounds into different frequencies.
link |
00:21:46.740
So you can think of the cochlea of your ear
link |
00:21:48.920
sort of like a prism.
link |
00:21:50.340
And then the brain takes that information
link |
00:21:52.620
and puts it back together and makes sense of it.
link |
00:21:55.100
So those hair cells in each of your two cochlea,
link |
00:21:58.160
because you have two ears, you also have two cochlea,
link |
00:22:01.320
send little wires, what we call axons,
link |
00:22:04.860
that convey their patterns of activity into the brain.
link |
00:22:09.580
And there are a number of different stations
link |
00:22:12.180
within the brain that information arrives at
link |
00:22:14.780
before it gets up to the parts of your brain
link |
00:22:17.100
where you are consciously aware.
link |
00:22:19.100
And because some of you have asked for more names
link |
00:22:21.240
in nomenclature, I'll give that to you.
link |
00:22:22.620
If you don't want a lot of detailed names,
link |
00:22:25.140
you can just ignore what I'm about to say.
link |
00:22:26.740
But basically the cochlea send information
link |
00:22:29.940
to what's called the spiral ganglion.
link |
00:22:32.300
The spiral ganglion, a ganglion, by the way,
link |
00:22:34.260
if you're going to learn any neuroscience,
link |
00:22:35.820
just know that anytime you hear ganglion,
link |
00:22:37.980
a ganglion is just a clump.
link |
00:22:40.100
So it means a bunch of neurons.
link |
00:22:41.900
So a clump of cells.
link |
00:22:43.580
So the spiral ganglion is a bunch of neurons
link |
00:22:46.140
that the information then goes off to
link |
00:22:48.540
what are called the cochlear nuclei in the brainstem.
link |
00:22:51.740
Brainstem is kind of down near your neck.
link |
00:22:53.980
Then up to a structure that has a really cool name
link |
00:22:55.860
called the superior olive
link |
00:22:57.220
because you have one on each side of your brain.
link |
00:22:59.780
And if I were to bring you to my lab
link |
00:23:01.900
and show you the superior olives in your brain
link |
00:23:04.340
or anyone else's brain, they look like little olives.
link |
00:23:07.180
They even have a little divot in them
link |
00:23:08.400
that to me looks like a pimento,
link |
00:23:10.620
but they just call them the superior olive.
link |
00:23:13.300
And then the neurons in the superior olive,
link |
00:23:15.540
then they send information up to what's called
link |
00:23:17.380
the inferior colliculus, only called inferior
link |
00:23:19.620
because it sits below a structure
link |
00:23:21.220
called the superior colliculus.
link |
00:23:22.700
And then the information goes up
link |
00:23:24.540
to what's called the medial geniculate nucleus
link |
00:23:27.340
and then up to your neocortex
link |
00:23:28.880
where you make sense of it all.
link |
00:23:31.460
Now you don't have to remember all that,
link |
00:23:32.900
but you should know that there are a lot of stations
link |
00:23:36.620
in which auditory information is processed
link |
00:23:39.100
before it gets up to our conscious detection.
link |
00:23:43.560
And there is a good reason for that,
link |
00:23:45.880
which is that more important than knowing what you're hearing
link |
00:23:50.660
you need to know where it's coming from.
link |
00:23:54.460
It's vital to our survival that if something,
link |
00:23:57.140
for instance, is falling toward us,
link |
00:23:59.080
that we know if it's coming to our right side,
link |
00:24:01.120
if it's going to hit us from behind,
link |
00:24:03.220
we have to know, for instance,
link |
00:24:04.260
if a car is coming at us from our left or from our right,
link |
00:24:07.620
and our visual system can help with that,
link |
00:24:10.460
but our auditory and our visual system collaborate
link |
00:24:13.020
to help us find and locate the position of things in space.
link |
00:24:17.820
That should come as no surprise.
link |
00:24:19.500
If you hear somebody talking off to your right,
link |
00:24:22.380
you tend to turn to your right, not to your left.
link |
00:24:24.780
If you see somebody's mouth moving in front of you,
link |
00:24:28.220
you tend to assume that the sound
link |
00:24:29.440
is going to come from right in front of you.
link |
00:24:32.220
Disruptions in this auditory hearing and visual matching
link |
00:24:37.860
are actually the basis of what's called
link |
00:24:39.660
the ventriloquism effect,
link |
00:24:41.260
which we'll talk about in a few minutes in more depth,
link |
00:24:44.100
but the ventriloquism effect can basically be described
link |
00:24:47.460
in simple terms as when you essentially think
link |
00:24:51.720
that a sound is coming from a location
link |
00:24:54.100
that it's not actually coming from.
link |
00:24:57.260
We'll talk about that in a moment,
link |
00:24:58.420
but what I'd like you to realize
link |
00:25:01.060
is that one of these stations deep in your brainstem
link |
00:25:05.020
is responsible for helping you identify
link |
00:25:07.340
where sounds are coming from
link |
00:25:09.480
through a process that's called interaural time differences.
link |
00:25:12.880
And that sounds fancy,
link |
00:25:13.980
but really the way you know where things are coming from,
link |
00:25:17.980
what direction a car or a bus or a person is coming from
link |
00:25:21.280
is because the sound lands in one ear before the other.
link |
00:25:25.060
And you have stations in your brain,
link |
00:25:27.660
meaning you have neurons in your brain
link |
00:25:29.180
that calculate the difference in time of arrival
link |
00:25:31.860
for those sound waves in your right versus your left ear.
link |
00:25:35.340
And if they arrive at the same time,
link |
00:25:37.240
you assume that thing is making noise right in front of you.
link |
00:25:40.980
If it's off to your right,
link |
00:25:42.220
you assume it's over on your right.
link |
00:25:43.740
And if the sound arrives first to your left ear,
link |
00:25:45.980
you assume quite correctly
link |
00:25:47.820
that the thing is coming toward your left ear.
link |
00:25:50.940
So it's a very simple and kind of mechanical system
link |
00:25:53.740
at the level of sound localization.
link |
00:25:57.900
But what about up and down?
link |
00:25:59.260
If you think about it,
link |
00:26:00.240
a sound coming from above is going to land on your right ear
link |
00:26:02.860
and your left ear at the same time.
link |
00:26:04.660
A sound from below is going to land on your right ear
link |
00:26:07.740
and your left ear at the same time.
link |
00:26:09.520
So the way that we know where things are
link |
00:26:12.700
in terms of what's called elevation,
link |
00:26:15.220
where they are in the up and down plane
link |
00:26:19.020
is by the frequencies.
link |
00:26:21.300
The shape of your ears actually modifies the sound
link |
00:26:25.460
depending on whether or not it's coming straight at you
link |
00:26:27.780
from the floor or from high above.
link |
00:26:30.740
And so already at the level of your ears,
link |
00:26:34.340
you are taking information about the outside world
link |
00:26:37.140
and determining where that information is coming from.
link |
00:26:40.740
Now, this all happens very, very fast and subconscious,
link |
00:26:44.100
but now you know why.
link |
00:26:46.220
If people really want to hear something,
link |
00:26:48.700
they make a cup around their ear.
link |
00:26:51.620
They essentially make their ear
link |
00:26:53.280
into more of a fennec fox type ear.
link |
00:26:55.700
If you've ever seen those cute little fennec fox things,
link |
00:26:58.340
they have these big spiky ears.
link |
00:26:59.800
They kind of look like a French bulldog,
link |
00:27:01.440
although they're kind of the fox version of the French bulldog
link |
00:27:04.460
with these big, tall ears,
link |
00:27:06.100
and they have excellent sound localization.
link |
00:27:08.260
And so when people lean in with their hand like this,
link |
00:27:12.200
if you're listening to this,
link |
00:27:13.040
I'm just cupping my hand at my ear,
link |
00:27:15.220
I'm giving myself a bigger pinna.
link |
00:27:17.820
Okay, and if I do it on the left side, I can do this side.
link |
00:27:20.300
And if I really want to hear something,
link |
00:27:21.740
I do it on both sides, okay?
link |
00:27:23.620
So this isn't just gesturing.
link |
00:27:25.300
This actually serves a mechanical role.
link |
00:27:27.420
And actually, if you want to hear where things are coming from
link |
00:27:30.360
with a much greater degree of accuracy,
link |
00:27:33.700
this can actually help
link |
00:27:35.260
because you're capturing sound waves
link |
00:27:36.940
and funneling them better.
link |
00:27:39.160
It's really remarkable, this whole system.
link |
00:27:42.440
So you've got these two ears,
link |
00:27:43.660
and because of the differences in the timing
link |
00:27:45.820
of when things arrive in those two ears,
link |
00:27:47.580
as well as these differences in the frequencies
link |
00:27:50.540
that certain things sound, or I should say,
link |
00:27:53.940
the differences in the frequencies
link |
00:27:56.060
that arrive at your ears,
link |
00:27:57.280
depending on whether or not the thing is above you
link |
00:27:59.900
or right in front of you or below you,
link |
00:28:01.600
you're able to make out
link |
00:28:02.440
where things are in space pretty well.
link |
00:28:04.340
So now you're probably starting to realize
link |
00:28:06.120
that these two things on the side of our head
link |
00:28:07.600
that we call ears are there for a lot more
link |
00:28:10.720
than hanging earrings on or for other aesthetic purposes
link |
00:28:14.860
or for putting sunglasses on top of.
link |
00:28:17.620
They are very powerful devices
link |
00:28:21.260
for allowing us to capture sound waves from our environment.
link |
00:28:26.580
Now I have a question for you,
link |
00:28:28.620
which is, can you move your ears?
link |
00:28:31.980
Turns out that unlike other animals,
link |
00:28:35.520
humans are not terrifically good at moving their ears.
link |
00:28:38.900
Other animals can move their ears even independently.
link |
00:28:42.240
So Costello is pretty good at raising his ears,
link |
00:28:44.880
the two of them together.
link |
00:28:45.760
He can't really move his ears separately.
link |
00:28:48.280
Some dogs can do that really well.
link |
00:28:50.500
In fact, sighthounds and some scent hounds
link |
00:28:53.740
do that exquisitely well.
link |
00:28:55.480
Some animals like deer and other animals
link |
00:28:58.440
that really have very acute hearing
link |
00:29:02.080
will put one ear down to a very particular angle
link |
00:29:05.960
and will tilt the other one
link |
00:29:07.180
and they will actually capture information
link |
00:29:10.520
about two distant sound-making organisms.
link |
00:29:15.040
Those could be hunters coming after them
link |
00:29:16.940
or other animals coming after them.
link |
00:29:18.600
They are very good at doing this.
link |
00:29:20.200
We're not so good at it,
link |
00:29:21.440
but about 60% of people it's thought
link |
00:29:24.960
can move their ears consciously
link |
00:29:27.520
without having to touch their ears.
link |
00:29:29.240
So can you do that?
link |
00:29:30.680
Maybe you should try it.
link |
00:29:31.520
Ask someone to look at you
link |
00:29:32.920
and see whether or not you can do it.
link |
00:29:34.440
The typical distances that people can move it
link |
00:29:37.680
is usually no more than two or three millimeters.
link |
00:29:40.300
It's subtle, but can you flap your pinna
link |
00:29:42.940
with just using mental control?
link |
00:29:45.720
If you can, or if you can't,
link |
00:29:49.360
try looking all the way to your right
link |
00:29:52.180
or all the way to your left.
link |
00:29:53.560
Obviously, if you're driving a car
link |
00:29:54.920
or doing something or exercising,
link |
00:29:56.480
don't put yourself in danger right now,
link |
00:29:57.780
but if you move your eyes all the way to your left,
link |
00:30:01.380
which I'm doing now, or all the way to my right,
link |
00:30:04.020
you might feel a little bit of a contraction of the muscles.
link |
00:30:06.980
It's that control ear movement, all right?
link |
00:30:11.800
Now I want to ask you this.
link |
00:30:12.780
Can you raise one eyebrow?
link |
00:30:14.560
I'm not very good at it.
link |
00:30:15.640
I can do a little bit,
link |
00:30:16.580
but it's mostly by like cramping down my face on one side
link |
00:30:20.040
and I certainly can't raise my right eyebrow.
link |
00:30:22.280
I can only do my left eyebrow.
link |
00:30:24.600
I'm trying to talk while I'm doing this,
link |
00:30:25.640
so this is why it looks strange.
link |
00:30:27.040
People who can raise one eyebrow very easily
link |
00:30:31.300
almost always can move their ears
link |
00:30:33.840
without having to touch them.
link |
00:30:35.480
It's controlled by the same motor pathway.
link |
00:30:39.520
And there does seem to be a small
link |
00:30:41.440
but statistically significant sex difference
link |
00:30:44.200
in the ability to move one's ears.
link |
00:30:47.180
Typically, men can do this more than women can,
link |
00:30:50.480
although plenty of women can move their ears as well.
link |
00:30:53.080
Now, if you think that is all a little strange or off topic,
link |
00:30:57.920
it's not because what we're really talking about here
link |
00:31:01.400
is a system of the brain,
link |
00:31:04.240
but also of the body of the musculature
link |
00:31:06.220
for localizing things in space.
link |
00:31:08.400
And so you might find it interesting to note
link |
00:31:11.220
that one of the things that we share very closely
link |
00:31:14.640
with other primates, with non-human primates,
link |
00:31:17.960
like macaque monkeys and chimpanzees,
link |
00:31:20.480
if you look at their ears, their ears are remarkably similar
link |
00:31:24.960
to our ears, or rather,
link |
00:31:26.320
our ears are remarkably similar to their ears.
link |
00:31:29.900
The eyes of certain monkeys like macaque monkeys
link |
00:31:33.640
are remarkably similar to human eyes.
link |
00:31:36.920
This is one of the reasons why
link |
00:31:38.120
if you look at a baby macaque monkey,
link |
00:31:40.800
it has this unbelievably human element to it.
link |
00:31:44.300
But the ears of these primates is very similar to our ears,
link |
00:31:49.040
our ears similar to their ears.
link |
00:31:51.640
If you're interested in ear movements
link |
00:31:54.200
and what they could mean,
link |
00:31:56.440
and some of the things that ear movements correlate with
link |
00:31:59.240
in other aspects of our biology,
link |
00:32:01.540
there's a nice paper, actually, a scientific paper.
link |
00:32:04.700
The author's last name is Code, C-O-D-E.
link |
00:32:07.920
It was published in 1995.
link |
00:32:09.600
I'll give a reference to that.
link |
00:32:11.020
It's a review article that discusses
link |
00:32:13.480
some of the sex differences in ear movement control,
link |
00:32:16.920
as well as the relationship between ear movements
link |
00:32:18.920
and eye movements.
link |
00:32:20.160
And it's a pretty accessible paper.
link |
00:32:21.840
It's one that I think any of you
link |
00:32:23.520
who are interested in this topic could parse fairly easily.
link |
00:32:27.020
And there's some very interesting underlying biology
link |
00:32:29.860
and some theories as to why humans would have
link |
00:32:32.660
this so-called vestigial or ancient carryover of a system
link |
00:32:36.640
for moving our ears.
link |
00:32:38.780
Now, if ear movement seems strange,
link |
00:32:43.000
next I want to talk about a different feature
link |
00:32:45.760
of your hearing and ears
link |
00:32:47.760
that's even stranger,
link |
00:32:49.920
but that has some really interesting implications
link |
00:32:53.200
for your biology.
link |
00:32:55.680
And I'm guessing that you've not heard of this.
link |
00:33:01.080
What I'm about to describe are called
link |
00:33:02.800
autoacoustic emissions.
link |
00:33:05.160
And autoacoustic emissions, as the name suggests,
link |
00:33:08.200
are sounds that your ears make.
link |
00:33:12.220
Believe it or not, 70% of people
link |
00:33:15.680
make noises with their ears,
link |
00:33:18.240
but they don't actually detect them.
link |
00:33:20.380
Like I said, you've never heard of this.
link |
00:33:21.820
Okay, that's not what I mean.
link |
00:33:24.580
But what I do mean is that 70% of people's ears
link |
00:33:28.680
are making noise that's cast out of the ear.
link |
00:33:32.080
And these autoacoustic emissions
link |
00:33:33.560
actually can be detected by microphones.
link |
00:33:35.360
Sometimes they can be detected by other people in the room
link |
00:33:38.400
if they have very good hearing.
link |
00:33:40.480
Now, it turns out that women,
link |
00:33:43.580
or I should be technical here,
link |
00:33:45.160
females who report themselves as heterosexual
link |
00:33:49.820
have a higher frequency, not frequency of sound,
link |
00:33:52.660
but a higher frequency of autoacoustic emissions
link |
00:33:56.840
than do men who report themselves as heterosexual.
link |
00:34:00.440
Women who report themselves as homosexual or bisexual
link |
00:34:05.640
make fewer autoacoustic emissions than heterosexual women.
link |
00:34:10.640
These are data that come from Dennis McFadden's lab
link |
00:34:13.640
at the University of Texas, Austin.
link |
00:34:15.680
He actually discovered these
link |
00:34:17.480
what are called sexual dimorphisms
link |
00:34:19.360
and differences based on sexual orientation
link |
00:34:22.480
without looking for them.
link |
00:34:23.560
He was studying hearing.
link |
00:34:24.960
He's a auditory scientist.
link |
00:34:27.180
And people were coming into his laboratory
link |
00:34:29.080
and they were detecting these autoacoustic emissions
link |
00:34:31.400
and they started to notice the group differences
link |
00:34:35.060
in autoacoustic emissions.
link |
00:34:36.240
So they started asking people about their sex
link |
00:34:39.280
and about their sexual orientation.
link |
00:34:41.320
And these differences fell out of the data, as we say.
link |
00:34:45.560
And it's interesting because autoacoustic emissions
link |
00:34:48.440
are not something that we associate with sex
link |
00:34:50.880
or sexual dimorphism,
link |
00:34:52.360
but what these data really underscore is, first of all,
link |
00:34:55.600
a lot of us are making noises with our ears,
link |
00:34:57.760
some of us more than others,
link |
00:34:59.420
and that exposure to certain combinations
link |
00:35:03.560
of hormones during development
link |
00:35:05.320
are very likely shaping the way that our hearing apparati,
link |
00:35:09.680
meaning the cochlea and the pinna and all sorts of things,
link |
00:35:12.560
how those develop and how those function
link |
00:35:14.360
throughout the lifespan.
link |
00:35:15.620
We did do an episode on hormones and sexual development,
link |
00:35:19.160
which gets much deeper into the other effects
link |
00:35:21.920
that hormones have on the developing brain and body.
link |
00:35:24.380
If you want to check out that episode,
link |
00:35:26.680
we will put a link to it in the captions.
link |
00:35:29.820
So now I want to shift to talking about
link |
00:35:31.920
ways to leverage your hearing system, your auditory system,
link |
00:35:36.360
so that you can learn anything,
link |
00:35:37.880
not just auditory information,
link |
00:35:39.360
but anything faster.
link |
00:35:41.900
I get a lot of questions about so-called binaural beats.
link |
00:35:45.920
Binaural beats, as their name suggests,
link |
00:35:48.200
involve playing one frequency of sound to one ear
link |
00:35:52.480
and a different frequency of sound to the other ear.
link |
00:35:55.200
So it might be doon, doon, doon, doon
link |
00:35:58.280
to your right ear,
link |
00:35:59.200
and it might be doon, doon, doon, doon, doon, doon,
link |
00:36:01.600
to the left ear.
link |
00:36:03.060
And the idea is that the brain will take
link |
00:36:06.240
those two frequencies of sound
link |
00:36:09.320
and because the pathways that bring information
link |
00:36:12.260
from the ears into the brain eventually cross over,
link |
00:36:14.880
they actually share that information
link |
00:36:16.920
with both sides of the brain,
link |
00:36:18.640
that the brain will average that information
link |
00:36:21.520
and come up with a sort of intermediate frequency.
link |
00:36:25.000
And the rationale is that those intermediate frequencies
link |
00:36:28.340
place the brain into a state that is better for learning.
link |
00:36:32.480
And when I say better for learning,
link |
00:36:34.660
I want to be precise about what I mean.
link |
00:36:36.620
That could mean more focus for encoding
link |
00:36:39.400
or bringing the information in.
link |
00:36:42.800
As you may have heard me say before,
link |
00:36:44.720
we have to be alert and focused in order to learn.
link |
00:36:47.240
There is no passive learning
link |
00:36:48.440
unless we're little tiny infants.
link |
00:36:51.020
So can binaural beats make us more focused?
link |
00:36:54.760
Can binaural beats allow us to relax more if we're anxious?
link |
00:36:58.760
I know some people, they go to the dentist
link |
00:37:00.300
and the dentist offers binaural beats
link |
00:37:02.360
as they drill into your teeth and give root canals
link |
00:37:04.640
and things of that sort,
link |
00:37:05.600
probably causing some anxiety
link |
00:37:06.820
just describing those things right now.
link |
00:37:09.720
But those are available in many dental practices.
link |
00:37:14.840
Their binaural beats have been thought
link |
00:37:16.440
to increase creativity,
link |
00:37:18.480
or at least have been proposed to increase creativity.
link |
00:37:20.620
So what are the scientific data say about binaural beats?
link |
00:37:23.320
There are a number of different apps out there
link |
00:37:25.640
that offer binaural beats.
link |
00:37:27.000
There are a number of different programs.
link |
00:37:28.480
I think you can also even just find these
link |
00:37:30.120
on YouTube and on the internet,
link |
00:37:32.160
but typically it's an app and you'll program in
link |
00:37:34.560
a particular outcome that you want,
link |
00:37:36.120
more focused, more creative, fall asleep,
link |
00:37:38.400
less anxious, et cetera.
link |
00:37:40.500
So what are the scientific data say?
link |
00:37:42.400
So believe it or not, the science on binaural beats
link |
00:37:45.040
is actually quite extensive and very precise.
link |
00:37:48.500
So sound waves are measured typically
link |
00:37:51.960
in hertz or kilohertz.
link |
00:37:53.520
I know many of you aren't familiar
link |
00:37:54.780
with thinking about things in hertz or kilohertz,
link |
00:37:56.900
but again, just remember those waves on a pond,
link |
00:37:59.680
those ripples on a pond,
link |
00:38:01.780
if they're close together, then they are of high frequency.
link |
00:38:06.440
And if they're far apart, then they are low frequency.
link |
00:38:08.680
So when you hear more hertz,
link |
00:38:10.840
what you're essentially hearing is higher frequency, right?
link |
00:38:14.660
And so if it's many more kilohertz,
link |
00:38:16.320
then it's much higher frequency
link |
00:38:17.520
than if it's fewer hertz or kilohertz.
link |
00:38:19.520
And so you may have heard of these things
link |
00:38:22.120
as delta waves or theta waves or alpha waves
link |
00:38:25.160
or beta waves, et cetera.
link |
00:38:26.680
Delta waves would be big, slow waves, so low frequency.
link |
00:38:30.320
And indeed, there is quality evidence
link |
00:38:33.660
from peer-reviewed studies that are not sponsored
link |
00:38:36.060
by companies that make binaural beat apps
link |
00:38:38.800
that tell us that delta waves like one to four hertz,
link |
00:38:42.760
so very low frequency sounds, think Costello's snoring,
link |
00:38:46.280
can help in the transition to sleep and for staying asleep.
link |
00:38:51.280
And that theta rhythms,
link |
00:38:52.680
which are more like four to eight hertz,
link |
00:38:55.040
can bring the brain into a state of subtle sleep
link |
00:39:00.560
or meditation, so deeply relaxed, but not fully asleep.
link |
00:39:04.360
And then you can sort of ascend the staircase of findings
link |
00:39:07.000
here, so to speak, and you'll find evidence
link |
00:39:08.980
that alpha waves, eight to 13 hertz,
link |
00:39:10.760
can increase alertness to a moderate level.
link |
00:39:14.300
That's a great state for the brain to be in
link |
00:39:16.360
for recall of existing information, okay?
link |
00:39:20.440
And that beta waves, 15 to 20 hertz,
link |
00:39:23.420
are great for bringing the brain into focus states
link |
00:39:27.320
for sustained thought or for incorporating new information,
link |
00:39:31.660
and especially gamma waves, the highest frequency,
link |
00:39:34.100
the most frequent ripples of sound, so to speak,
link |
00:39:37.680
32 to 100 hertz for learning and problem solving.
link |
00:39:41.600
Now, all of this matches, or I should say maps
link |
00:39:44.920
onto what I've said before about learning really nicely,
link |
00:39:48.540
which is that you need to be in a highly alert state
link |
00:39:50.880
in order to bring new information in
link |
00:39:52.840
in order to access a state of mind
link |
00:39:55.440
in which you can tell your brain,
link |
00:39:57.600
or the brain is telling itself, okay, I need to learn this.
link |
00:39:59.880
This is why stress and unfortunate circumstances
link |
00:40:02.440
are so memorable is because our brain gets
link |
00:40:04.440
into a really high alert system.
link |
00:40:06.080
Here, we're talking about the use of binaural beats
link |
00:40:08.380
in order to increase our level of alertness
link |
00:40:11.200
or our level of calmness.
link |
00:40:14.140
Now, that's important to underscore
link |
00:40:16.260
because it's not that there's something
link |
00:40:17.720
fundamentally important about the binaural beats.
link |
00:40:20.780
They are yet another way of bringing the brain
link |
00:40:23.380
into states of deep relaxation through low-frequency sound
link |
00:40:27.480
or highly alert states for focused learning
link |
00:40:31.380
with more high-frequency sound.
link |
00:40:33.440
So they are effective,
link |
00:40:35.780
and I'll review a little bit of the data in detail.
link |
00:40:39.040
They're effective,
link |
00:40:40.040
but it's not that they're uniquely special for learning.
link |
00:40:44.920
It's just that they can help some people
link |
00:40:46.920
bring their brain into the state
link |
00:40:48.460
that allows them to learn better.
link |
00:40:50.820
So there are a lot of studies that allowed us to arrive,
link |
00:40:55.040
or I should say allowed the field to arrive
link |
00:40:57.720
on these parameters of slow, low-frequency waves
link |
00:41:02.640
are going to bring you into relaxed states,
link |
00:41:03.900
high-frequency waves into more alert states.
link |
00:41:08.220
There's very good evidence for anxiety reduction
link |
00:41:11.620
from the use of binaural beats.
link |
00:41:14.280
And what's interesting is the anxiety reduction
link |
00:41:18.300
seems to be most effective when the binaural beats
link |
00:41:22.980
are bringing the brain into delta,
link |
00:41:25.280
so those slow, big waves like sleep, theta, and alpha states.
link |
00:41:30.240
And I'll link to a couple of these studies,
link |
00:41:32.300
although I will probably link more to the list
link |
00:41:36.020
that really segregates them out one by one
link |
00:41:38.020
so you can see them all next to one another.
link |
00:41:40.120
There's good evidence that binaural beats
link |
00:41:41.580
can be used to treat pain, chronic pain.
link |
00:41:43.860
There's three studies in peer-reviewed journals,
link |
00:41:47.620
which I took a look at, and they seem to be of good quality,
link |
00:41:51.580
not sponsored research, as we say,
link |
00:41:53.280
not paid for by any specific company.
link |
00:41:55.940
Binaural beats have been shown
link |
00:41:57.140
to modestly improve cognition, attention,
link |
00:42:00.740
working memory, and even creativity.
link |
00:42:03.700
But the real boost from binaural beats
link |
00:42:06.560
appears to be for anxiety reduction and pain reduction.
link |
00:42:10.980
Some people might find these beneficial
link |
00:42:12.680
for these oral surgeries, right?
link |
00:42:16.100
Believe it or not, there are people who would rather
link |
00:42:18.800
have the entire root canal or cavity drilled
link |
00:42:22.660
without Novocain, and that's because they sometimes
link |
00:42:25.420
have a syringe phobia or something of that sort,
link |
00:42:28.340
or they just don't like being numb from the Novocain,
link |
00:42:30.120
or maybe there's an underlying medical reason.
link |
00:42:32.100
But I think most people don't enjoy
link |
00:42:34.340
getting their teeth drilled,
link |
00:42:35.300
even if they have Novocain in there or a root canal.
link |
00:42:37.700
And so it seems that binaural beats
link |
00:42:39.700
can be effective in that environment,
link |
00:42:41.700
and you don't have to go into that sort of
link |
00:42:43.220
extreme environment to benefit from binaural beats.
link |
00:42:45.240
Binaural beats are either
link |
00:42:49.020
relatively inexpensive thing to access.
link |
00:42:51.980
Most of the apps are pretty inexpensive.
link |
00:42:53.700
I don't have a favorite binaural beats app
link |
00:42:56.460
to recommend to you.
link |
00:42:57.880
I confess I did use binaural beats a few years ago.
link |
00:43:00.140
I kind of shifted over to other what I call
link |
00:43:03.500
NSDR, non-sleep deep-rest protocols, in favor of those.
link |
00:43:06.500
But many people like binaural beats
link |
00:43:08.580
and say that they benefit from them,
link |
00:43:10.720
especially while studying or learning.
link |
00:43:13.460
I think part of the reason for that
link |
00:43:15.540
relates to the ability to channel our focus
link |
00:43:19.340
when we have some background noise.
link |
00:43:21.380
And this is something I also get asked about a lot.
link |
00:43:24.140
Is it better to listen to music
link |
00:43:25.720
and have background noise when studying,
link |
00:43:27.820
or is it better to have complete silence?
link |
00:43:29.820
Well, there's actually a quite good literature on this
link |
00:43:33.140
as well, but not so much as it relates to binaural beats,
link |
00:43:36.100
but rather whether or not people are listening to music,
link |
00:43:39.380
so-called white noise, brown noise,
link |
00:43:42.460
believe it or not, there's white noise
link |
00:43:43.580
and there's brown noise, there's even pink noise,
link |
00:43:46.100
and how that impacts brain states
link |
00:43:49.120
that allow us to learn information better or not.
link |
00:43:52.340
So now I'd like to talk about white noise.
link |
00:43:55.100
And I want to be very clear that white noise has been shown
link |
00:43:58.940
to really enhance brain states for learning
link |
00:44:02.480
in certain individuals, in particular in adults.
link |
00:44:06.460
But white noise actually can have a detrimental effect
link |
00:44:09.460
on auditory learning and maybe even the development
link |
00:44:12.020
of the auditory system in very young children,
link |
00:44:14.900
in particular in infants.
link |
00:44:16.940
So first I'd like to talk about the beneficial effects
link |
00:44:19.880
of white noise on learning.
link |
00:44:22.280
There are some really excellent studies on this.
link |
00:44:25.460
The first one that I'd like to just highlight
link |
00:44:28.540
is one that's entitled Low-Intensity White Noise
link |
00:44:31.740
Improves Performance in Auditory Working Memory Task,
link |
00:44:34.420
an FMRI study.
link |
00:44:35.980
This is a study that explored whether or not learning
link |
00:44:40.980
could be enhanced by playing white noise in the background.
link |
00:44:46.100
But the strength of the study is that they looked
link |
00:44:48.860
at some of the underlying neural circuitry
link |
00:44:50.620
and the activation of the neural circuitry in these people
link |
00:44:52.800
as they did the learning task.
link |
00:44:54.380
And what it essentially illustrates is that white noise,
link |
00:44:58.220
provided that white noise is of low enough intensity,
link |
00:45:01.980
meaning not super loud, right?
link |
00:45:04.880
Not imperceptible, so not so quiet that you can't hear it,
link |
00:45:09.100
but not super loud either.
link |
00:45:10.740
It actually could enhance learning to a significant degree.
link |
00:45:13.740
And this has been shown now for a huge number
link |
00:45:16.540
of different types of learning.
link |
00:45:19.860
There's a terrific article as well
link |
00:45:21.940
as in a somewhat obscure journal, at least obscure to me,
link |
00:45:24.540
which is the effects of noise exposure
link |
00:45:26.240
on cognitive performance and brain activity patterns.
link |
00:45:30.260
That's a study involving 54 subjects.
link |
00:45:33.540
They basically were evaluated for mental workload
link |
00:45:36.400
and attention under different levels of noise exposure,
link |
00:45:39.700
background noise, and different essentially loudness
link |
00:45:43.020
of noise.
link |
00:45:43.860
And the reason I like this study is that they looked
link |
00:45:45.740
at different levels of noise and types of noise,
link |
00:45:47.940
and they varied a number of different things
link |
00:45:50.460
as opposed to just doing a kind of two-condition,
link |
00:45:52.780
either white noise or no white noise type thing.
link |
00:45:55.260
And what they found, again, is that provided
link |
00:45:57.620
the white noise is not extremely loud,
link |
00:46:00.680
it could really enhance brain function
link |
00:46:02.920
for sake of learning any number
link |
00:46:04.500
of different kinds of information.
link |
00:46:06.420
Now, that's all great, but it really doesn't get
link |
00:46:10.420
to the kind of deeper guts of mechanism.
link |
00:46:12.900
And as a neuroscientist, what I really want to see
link |
00:46:15.100
is not just that something has an effect.
link |
00:46:16.880
That's always nice.
link |
00:46:17.720
It's always nice to see in a nice peer-reviewed study
link |
00:46:20.320
without any kind of commercial biases
link |
00:46:22.340
that there's an effect.
link |
00:46:23.220
Okay, binaural beats can enhance learning,
link |
00:46:25.260
or listening to white noise not too loud
link |
00:46:27.480
can enhance learning.
link |
00:46:28.940
But you really want to understand mechanism
link |
00:46:31.580
because once you understand mechanism,
link |
00:46:34.760
not only does it start to make sense,
link |
00:46:36.300
but you can also imagine ways
link |
00:46:37.660
in which you could develop better tools and protocols.
link |
00:46:40.660
So I was very relieved to find,
link |
00:46:43.500
or I should say excited to find this study
link |
00:46:45.820
published in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
link |
00:46:48.460
This is a 2014 paper.
link |
00:46:51.700
White noise improves learning by modulating activity
link |
00:46:54.700
in dopaminergic midbrain regions
link |
00:46:57.380
and the right superior temporal sulcus, okay?
link |
00:46:59.620
Now, I don't expect you to know
link |
00:47:00.500
what the right superior temporal sulcus is.
link |
00:47:02.460
I don't expect you to know
link |
00:47:03.500
what the dopamine midbrain region is,
link |
00:47:05.260
but if you're like me,
link |
00:47:07.260
you probably took highlighted notice
link |
00:47:09.740
of the word dopaminergic.
link |
00:47:11.220
Dopamine is a neuromodulator,
link |
00:47:13.660
meaning it's a chemical that's released in our brain
link |
00:47:15.840
and body, but mostly in our brain,
link |
00:47:18.020
that modulates, meaning controls the likelihood
link |
00:47:23.100
that certain brain areas will be active
link |
00:47:24.780
and other brain areas won't be active.
link |
00:47:26.480
And dopamine is associated with motivation.
link |
00:47:28.740
Dopamine is associated with craving.
link |
00:47:30.500
Motivation is associated with all sorts of different things,
link |
00:47:32.800
including movement.
link |
00:47:34.220
But what this study so nicely shows
link |
00:47:38.100
is that white noise can really enhance
link |
00:47:42.220
the activity of neurons
link |
00:47:43.460
in what's called the substantia nigra VTA.
link |
00:47:45.720
The substantia nigra VTA is a very rich source of dopamine,
link |
00:47:50.220
and that's because it's very chock-a-block
link |
00:47:53.400
full of dopamine neurons.
link |
00:47:55.500
It's an area of the brain
link |
00:47:57.000
that is perhaps the richest source of dopamine neurons.
link |
00:48:01.260
And you actually can see this brain region
link |
00:48:03.740
under the microscope.
link |
00:48:04.720
If you take a slice of brain or you look at a brain
link |
00:48:07.100
without even staining it for any proteins
link |
00:48:09.060
or dopamine or anything,
link |
00:48:10.460
it's two very dark regions
link |
00:48:12.780
at the kind of bottom of the brain.
link |
00:48:14.700
And the reason it's called substantia nigra,
link |
00:48:17.500
nigra meaning dark,
link |
00:48:19.500
is because the dopamine neurons actually make something
link |
00:48:23.180
and that makes those neurons dark.
link |
00:48:25.740
And so you've got these two regions down there
link |
00:48:28.260
that contain dopamine and can release dopamine
link |
00:48:30.560
and essentially activate other brain regions
link |
00:48:32.960
and activate our sense of motivation
link |
00:48:34.680
and activate our sense of desire
link |
00:48:36.700
to continue focusing and learning.
link |
00:48:39.020
But you can't just snap your fingers
link |
00:48:41.460
and make them release dopamine.
link |
00:48:42.500
You actually have to trigger dopamine release from them.
link |
00:48:44.940
Now, that trigger can be caused
link |
00:48:46.520
by being very excited about something
link |
00:48:49.080
or the fact that that thing gave you
link |
00:48:50.780
a lot of pleasure in the past
link |
00:48:52.160
or you're highly motivated by fear or desire.
link |
00:48:55.160
But what's so interesting to me is that it appears
link |
00:48:58.600
that white noise itself can raise
link |
00:49:02.020
the what we call the basal,
link |
00:49:03.380
the baseline levels of dopamine
link |
00:49:05.280
that are being released from this area,
link |
00:49:07.440
the substantia nigra.
link |
00:49:09.260
So now we're starting to get a more full picture
link |
00:49:12.980
of how particular sounds in our environment
link |
00:49:15.660
can increase learning.
link |
00:49:18.260
And that's in part, I believe,
link |
00:49:20.420
through the release of dopamine from substantia nigra.
link |
00:49:24.700
So I'm not trying to shift you away from binaural beats
link |
00:49:28.220
if that's your thing,
link |
00:49:29.280
but it does appear that turning on white noise
link |
00:49:32.220
at a low level, not too loud,
link |
00:49:33.860
you may say, well, how loud?
link |
00:49:35.100
And I'll tell you in a moment,
link |
00:49:36.140
but not too loud can allow you to learn better
link |
00:49:39.360
because of the ways that it's modulating
link |
00:49:41.280
your brain chemistry.
link |
00:49:42.640
So how loud or how soft should that white noise be
link |
00:49:47.040
while you learn?
link |
00:49:48.900
Well, in these studies, it seemed that white noise
link |
00:49:52.460
that could be heard by the person,
link |
00:49:54.300
so it wasn't imperceptible to them,
link |
00:49:55.820
so it was loud enough that they could hear,
link |
00:49:58.740
but not so loud that they felt it was intrusive
link |
00:50:03.860
or irritating to them.
link |
00:50:05.780
So that's going to differ from person to person
link |
00:50:07.640
because people have different levels of auditory sensitivity.
link |
00:50:10.860
It's going to depend on age,
link |
00:50:11.820
going to depend on a number of different factors.
link |
00:50:13.800
So I can't tell you turn to level two
link |
00:50:16.600
on your volume controller.
link |
00:50:17.900
That's just not going to work.
link |
00:50:18.740
Also, I don't know how far you are
link |
00:50:20.080
from a given speaker in the room,
link |
00:50:21.580
or if you've got earphones in your head,
link |
00:50:23.440
or you've got speakers in the room,
link |
00:50:26.060
or if it's coming out of your computer.
link |
00:50:27.760
I don't know those things.
link |
00:50:28.960
So what you're going to have to do is adjust that white noise
link |
00:50:31.420
to a place where it's not interfering
link |
00:50:33.860
with your ability to focus,
link |
00:50:35.080
but rather it's enhancing your ability to focus.
link |
00:50:37.820
I think a good rule of thumb is going to be
link |
00:50:41.200
to put it probably on the lower third
link |
00:50:43.860
of any kind of volume dial,
link |
00:50:46.080
as opposed to in the upper third
link |
00:50:49.160
where it would really be blasting.
link |
00:50:51.500
And really blasting any noise, frankly, is not good,
link |
00:50:55.840
but that's especially not good,
link |
00:50:58.200
meaning it's especially bad, if you have headphones in.
link |
00:51:01.440
I do want to mention something about headphones
link |
00:51:03.460
before I talk about white noise in the developmental context
link |
00:51:07.000
and why it can be dangerous there.
link |
00:51:09.580
When you put headphones in your ears,
link |
00:51:13.760
it has this incredible effect of making the sounds
link |
00:51:17.720
like they come from inside your head,
link |
00:51:19.680
not from out in the room.
link |
00:51:21.640
And now that might seem like kind of a duh,
link |
00:51:23.720
but that's actually really amazing, right?
link |
00:51:25.880
Your brain assumes that the sounds are coming
link |
00:51:28.080
from inside your head,
link |
00:51:30.120
as opposed from the environment that you're in
link |
00:51:32.020
the moment you put headphones in.
link |
00:51:33.600
So if you're listening to an audio book,
link |
00:51:34.940
or maybe you're listening to this podcast with headphones,
link |
00:51:37.240
that's very different than when you're listening
link |
00:51:39.100
to something out in the room and there are other sounds,
link |
00:51:41.640
other sound waves,
link |
00:51:42.600
especially if you use these noise cancellation headphones.
link |
00:51:45.200
So if you're going to use white noise
link |
00:51:48.120
to enhance studying or learning of any kind,
link |
00:51:50.560
or this also could be for skill learning,
link |
00:51:52.000
motor skill learning while you're exercising,
link |
00:51:54.640
my suggestion would be that if you're using headphones
link |
00:51:58.200
to keep it quite low, right?
link |
00:52:00.280
This is an effect on the midbrain dopamine neurons.
link |
00:52:03.380
That's a background effect of raising the baseline
link |
00:52:05.840
of dopamine release.
link |
00:52:06.680
The way that dopamine neurons fires, they're always firing.
link |
00:52:08.880
Yours are firing right now, so are mine.
link |
00:52:10.680
When something exciting happens, they fire a lot.
link |
00:52:12.960
And when something disappointing happens,
link |
00:52:14.360
that firing, the release of dopamine
link |
00:52:16.280
goes down below baseline.
link |
00:52:17.960
What you're talking about here is raising
link |
00:52:19.560
your overall levels of attention and motivation,
link |
00:52:21.620
which translate to better learning,
link |
00:52:23.440
by just tickling those neurons a little bit,
link |
00:52:26.280
raising the baseline firing, okay?
link |
00:52:28.100
So you're not turning up the white noise
link |
00:52:30.280
to the point where you're feeling amazing.
link |
00:52:32.040
This isn't like turning on your favorite song.
link |
00:52:34.080
This is actually the opposite.
link |
00:52:35.800
This is about getting that baseline up just a bit, okay?
link |
00:52:39.820
So I recommend turning the volume up just a bit
link |
00:52:43.420
so that you can focus entirely on the tasks
link |
00:52:46.720
that you're trying to do.
link |
00:52:47.560
And of course, you've turned on white noise,
link |
00:52:49.240
so your attention might drift to that for a moment.
link |
00:52:50.860
Is it too loud? Is it too soft?
link |
00:52:52.840
If you can disappear into the work, so to speak,
link |
00:52:55.280
if your attention can disappear into the work,
link |
00:52:57.140
then that's probably sufficiently quiet.
link |
00:53:00.280
And for those of you that say,
link |
00:53:01.200
well, I like really loud music,
link |
00:53:02.560
and if I just blast the music,
link |
00:53:04.400
then I forget about the music.
link |
00:53:06.820
I don't suggest blasting music.
link |
00:53:08.520
And this is coming from somebody
link |
00:53:09.520
who really likes loud music.
link |
00:53:12.200
I grew up with kind of a loud, fast rules mentality.
link |
00:53:14.640
And if you don't know what loud, fast rules means,
link |
00:53:17.400
then I can't help you.
link |
00:53:18.740
But there's a time and a place, perhaps,
link |
00:53:22.460
to listen to music loud.
link |
00:53:23.620
But especially with headphones,
link |
00:53:25.500
you can trigger, excuse me, hearing loss quite rapidly.
link |
00:53:30.000
And unfortunately, because these hair cells
link |
00:53:33.180
that we talked about earlier,
link |
00:53:34.120
our central nervous system neurons,
link |
00:53:35.640
they do not regenerate.
link |
00:53:37.120
They do not come back.
link |
00:53:39.000
Now, along the lines of hearing loss,
link |
00:53:43.000
I should just say that the best way
link |
00:53:45.200
to blow out your hearing for good, to eliminate your hearing,
link |
00:53:49.000
is to have very loud sounds
link |
00:53:51.880
superimposed on a loud environment, okay?
link |
00:53:54.840
So loud environments can cause hearing loss over time.
link |
00:53:57.400
So if you work at a construction site clanging really loud,
link |
00:54:00.300
or if you work the soundboard in a club or something,
link |
00:54:03.700
you are headed towards hearing loss
link |
00:54:05.720
unless you protect your hearing
link |
00:54:07.600
with earplugs and headphones.
link |
00:54:10.000
Nowadays, some of the earplugs are very low profile,
link |
00:54:12.800
meaning you can't see them.
link |
00:54:13.840
So that's kind of nice so you're not like the,
link |
00:54:15.960
like when I was younger,
link |
00:54:16.980
you didn't want to be the dork
link |
00:54:17.940
to go to the concert with the earplugs,
link |
00:54:19.360
but turns out those dorks were smarter than everybody else
link |
00:54:21.800
because they're not the ones who are craning their neck
link |
00:54:24.720
to try and hear trivial things
link |
00:54:27.940
at the age of 30 or so
link |
00:54:30.600
because they blew out their hearing.
link |
00:54:32.440
So if you are working in a loud environment
link |
00:54:35.340
or you expose yourselves to a loud environment,
link |
00:54:38.660
you really want to avoid big inflections
link |
00:54:41.960
and sound above that.
link |
00:54:43.360
So loud environment plus fireworks,
link |
00:54:47.200
loud environment plus gunshot,
link |
00:54:49.140
loud environments plus very high frequency, intense sound.
link |
00:54:53.560
That's what we call the two-hip model.
link |
00:54:55.560
When you, this is also true for concussion
link |
00:54:57.800
that you can take a kind of a stimulus
link |
00:54:59.840
that normally would be below the threshold of injury.
link |
00:55:02.040
You add another stimulus at the same time
link |
00:55:05.140
that would be below the threshold of injury
link |
00:55:06.820
and then suddenly you killed the neurons.
link |
00:55:08.800
So I don't want to make people paranoid,
link |
00:55:10.560
but you do want to protect your hearing.
link |
00:55:11.860
It's no fun to lose your hearing.
link |
00:55:13.600
If you're going to use headphones
link |
00:55:15.160
and you feel like you want to crank it up all the way,
link |
00:55:17.040
just remember that the more that you can get
link |
00:55:20.740
out of a lower volume,
link |
00:55:22.060
meaning the longer that you can go listening to things
link |
00:55:24.720
at lower volume,
link |
00:55:25.640
the longer you'll be able to hear that music or that thing.
link |
00:55:30.120
So again, I'm not the hearing cop.
link |
00:55:33.840
That's not my job,
link |
00:55:35.180
but as somebody who's lost some of his high-frequency
link |
00:55:38.120
hearing, I can tell you it's not a pleasure.
link |
00:55:42.200
The old argument that it helps you not have to hear
link |
00:55:45.480
or listen to people that you don't want to listen to
link |
00:55:47.280
that doesn't really work.
link |
00:55:48.340
They just send you text messages instead.
link |
00:55:50.360
So what about white noise and hearing loss in development?
link |
00:55:53.960
You know, a lot of people with children
link |
00:55:55.340
have these kind of noise machines,
link |
00:55:57.560
like sound waves and things like that
link |
00:55:59.240
that help the kids sleep.
link |
00:56:00.960
And look, I think kids getting good sleep
link |
00:56:03.620
and parents getting good sleep is vital to physical
link |
00:56:06.520
and mental health and family health.
link |
00:56:08.960
So I certainly sympathize with those needs.
link |
00:56:13.880
However, there are data that indicate that white noise
link |
00:56:18.360
during development can be detrimental
link |
00:56:21.000
to the auditory system.
link |
00:56:22.400
I don't want to frighten any parents.
link |
00:56:23.720
If you played white noise to your kids,
link |
00:56:25.480
this doesn't mean that their auditory system
link |
00:56:27.060
or their speech patterns are going to be disrupted
link |
00:56:29.760
or that their interpretation of speech
link |
00:56:31.120
is going to be disrupted forever.
link |
00:56:32.880
But there are data published in the journal Science,
link |
00:56:37.120
and Science being one of the three apex journals,
link |
00:56:39.600
Science, Nature, Cell, the most stringent journals,
link |
00:56:43.360
data published in the journal Science some years ago,
link |
00:56:46.180
actually by a scientist who I know quite well.
link |
00:56:48.280
His name is Edward Chang.
link |
00:56:49.280
He's a medical doctor now.
link |
00:56:50.800
He's a neurosurgeon.
link |
00:56:51.920
He's actually the chair of neurosurgery at UCSF.
link |
00:56:55.080
And he runs a laboratory where they study auditory learning,
link |
00:56:58.840
neuroplasticity, et cetera.
link |
00:57:00.440
He and his mentor at the time, Mike Merzenich,
link |
00:57:04.740
published a paper showing that if young animals,
link |
00:57:08.600
and this wasn't animal models,
link |
00:57:10.460
were exposed to white noise, so shh,
link |
00:57:14.980
the very type of noise that I'm saying as a older person,
link |
00:57:18.960
and when I say older,
link |
00:57:19.800
I mean somebody who's in their late teens,
link |
00:57:21.920
early 20s and older,
link |
00:57:23.520
could benefit from listening to that at a low level
link |
00:57:25.880
in the background for sake of learning.
link |
00:57:28.500
Well, they exposed very young animals to this white noise.
link |
00:57:32.040
It actually disrupted the maps
link |
00:57:34.660
of the auditory world within the brain.
link |
00:57:37.680
And we haven't talked about these maps yet,
link |
00:57:39.240
but I want to take a moment and talk about them
link |
00:57:40.640
and explain this effect and what it might mean for you
link |
00:57:44.000
if you have kids or if you were exposed
link |
00:57:45.560
to a lot of white noise early on.
link |
00:57:47.640
So auditory information goes up into our cortex,
link |
00:57:52.280
into essentially the outside portion of our brain
link |
00:57:55.760
that's responsible for all of our higher level cognition,
link |
00:57:59.520
our planning, our decision-making, et cetera, creativity.
link |
00:58:03.220
And up there, we have what are called tonotopic maps.
link |
00:58:06.920
What's a tonotopic map?
link |
00:58:08.400
Well, remember the cochlea, how it's coiled,
link |
00:58:10.720
and at one end, it responds to high frequencies,
link |
00:58:12.680
and the other end, it responds to low frequencies?
link |
00:58:15.520
Sort of like a piano.
link |
00:58:16.720
The keys sound different as you extend down
link |
00:58:19.280
and up the piano keys,
link |
00:58:20.960
and it's organized in a very systematic way, right?
link |
00:58:23.680
It's not all intermixed high frequencies and low frequencies.
link |
00:58:26.720
It's organized in a very systematic way
link |
00:58:29.040
from one end to the other.
link |
00:58:30.700
Your visual system is in what's called a retinotopic map.
link |
00:58:33.640
So neighboring points in space off to my right,
link |
00:58:36.840
like my two fingers off to my right,
link |
00:58:38.880
are mapped to neighboring points in space in my brain.
link |
00:58:43.000
And the space right in front of me
link |
00:58:45.400
is mapped to a different location in my brain,
link |
00:58:47.160
but it's systematic.
link |
00:58:48.240
It's regular.
link |
00:58:49.080
It's not random.
link |
00:58:49.900
It's not salt and pepper.
link |
00:58:51.040
It goes from high to low, or from right to center to left.
link |
00:58:54.960
In the auditory system,
link |
00:58:56.080
we have what are called tonotopic maps,
link |
00:58:58.460
where frequency, high frequency to low frequency,
link |
00:59:02.320
and everything in between is organized
link |
00:59:04.240
in a very systematic way.
link |
00:59:06.340
Now, our experience of life from the time we're a baby
link |
00:59:09.520
until the time that we die is not systematic.
link |
00:59:12.640
We don't hear low frequencies at one part of the room
link |
00:59:15.160
or at one part of the day,
link |
00:59:16.040
and high frequencies at another part of the room
link |
00:59:17.660
and another part of the day.
link |
00:59:18.840
They're all intermixed.
link |
00:59:20.400
But if you remember, the cochlea separates them out
link |
00:59:23.480
just like a prism of light separates out
link |
00:59:25.560
the different wavelengths of light.
link |
00:59:26.740
The cochlea separates out the different frequencies.
link |
00:59:29.980
And the developing brain
link |
00:59:32.300
takes those separated out frequencies
link |
00:59:34.680
and learns this relationship between itself,
link |
00:59:39.280
meaning the child, and the outside world.
link |
00:59:43.440
White noise essentially contains no tonotopic information.
link |
00:59:48.440
The frequencies are all intermixed.
link |
00:59:51.280
It's just noise.
link |
00:59:52.840
Whereas when I speak, my voice has,
link |
00:59:55.400
now I'm getting technical,
link |
00:59:56.240
but it has what's called a certain envelope,
link |
00:59:58.480
meaning it has some low frequencies
link |
01:00:00.080
and some slightly high frequencies.
link |
01:00:01.440
I can make my voice higher,
link |
01:00:02.760
although I'm not very good at that.
link |
01:00:04.040
My voice starts to crack and I can make my voice lower,
link |
01:00:07.200
although not as low as Costello's snore.
link |
01:00:08.860
So it has an envelope, it has a container.
link |
01:00:12.280
White noise has no container.
link |
01:00:14.000
It's like all the colors of the rainbow spread out together,
link |
01:00:17.400
which is actually what you get when you get white light.
link |
01:00:19.860
White noise is analogous to white light.
link |
01:00:23.400
So one of the reasons why hearing a lot of white noise
link |
01:00:29.600
during development for long periods of time
link |
01:00:31.600
can be detrimental to the development of the auditory system
link |
01:00:34.360
is that these tonotopic maps don't form normally.
link |
01:00:37.120
At least they don't in experimental animals.
link |
01:00:39.040
Now, the reason I'm raising this is that many people I know,
link |
01:00:43.400
in particular friends who have small children,
link |
01:00:45.800
they say, I want to use a white noise machine while I sleep,
link |
01:00:51.980
but is it okay for my baby to use a white noise machine?
link |
01:00:56.160
And I consulted with various people, scientists about this,
link |
01:00:59.740
and they said, well, you know,
link |
01:01:02.080
the baby is also hearing the parents' voices
link |
01:01:04.360
and is hearing music and is hearing the dog bark,
link |
01:01:06.520
so it's not the only thing they're hearing.
link |
01:01:08.320
However, every single person that I consulted with said,
link |
01:01:12.780
but you know, there's neuroplasticity during sleep,
link |
01:01:15.800
that's when the kid is sleeping,
link |
01:01:17.640
and I don't know that you'd want to expose a child
link |
01:01:20.460
to white noise the entire night,
link |
01:01:22.560
because it might degrade that tonotopic map.
link |
01:01:26.320
It might not destroy it, it might not eliminate it,
link |
01:01:28.880
but it could make it a little less clear,
link |
01:01:32.000
like sort of taking the keys on the piano
link |
01:01:34.960
and taping a few of them together, right?
link |
01:01:37.360
So you've still got the highs and lows
link |
01:01:38.720
in the appropriate order and everything in between,
link |
01:01:41.080
but when you tape the keys together,
link |
01:01:42.720
you don't get the same fidelity,
link |
01:01:44.320
you don't get the same precision
link |
01:01:46.360
of the noise that comes out of that piano.
link |
01:01:49.760
So again, I don't want to scare anybody,
link |
01:01:52.920
but I would say if you are in a position
link |
01:01:54.600
to make the choice of either using white noise
link |
01:01:57.600
or something similar,
link |
01:01:58.600
pink noise is just a kind of variation,
link |
01:02:00.280
it's got a little bit more of a certain frequency,
link |
01:02:02.280
just like pink light has a little bit more
link |
01:02:04.200
of a certain wavelength than white light, kind of,
link |
01:02:07.180
if you are in a position to make choices
link |
01:02:11.020
about things to put in a young,
link |
01:02:13.860
especially very young child's sleeping environment,
link |
01:02:16.400
white noise might be something to consider avoiding.
link |
01:02:19.680
Again, I'm not telling you what to do,
link |
01:02:21.200
but it's something to perhaps consider avoiding.
link |
01:02:23.740
I don't think most pediatricians
link |
01:02:25.400
are going to be aware of these data,
link |
01:02:27.300
but if you talk to any auditory physiologist
link |
01:02:30.200
or an audiologist or somebody
link |
01:02:31.960
who studies auditory development,
link |
01:02:34.280
I'm fairly certain that they would have opinions about that.
link |
01:02:37.200
Now, whether or not their opinions agree with mine
link |
01:02:39.440
and these folks that I consulted with
link |
01:02:40.840
or not is a separate matter,
link |
01:02:42.640
I don't know because I don't know them,
link |
01:02:44.960
but it's something that I felt was important enough
link |
01:02:47.280
to cue you to, especially since I've highlighted,
link |
01:02:51.440
excuse me, the opposite effect is true in adulthood.
link |
01:02:54.080
Once your auditory system has formed,
link |
01:02:56.460
once it's established these tonotopic maps,
link |
01:02:58.640
then the presence of background white noise
link |
01:03:01.160
should not be a problem at all.
link |
01:03:02.720
In fact, it shouldn't be a problem at all
link |
01:03:05.240
because you're also not attending to it.
link |
01:03:07.300
The idea is that it's playing at a low enough volume
link |
01:03:09.460
that you kind of forget it in the background
link |
01:03:11.080
and that it's supporting learning
link |
01:03:12.640
by bringing your brain into a heightened state of alertness
link |
01:03:15.200
and especially this heightened state of dopamine,
link |
01:03:17.640
dopaminergic activation of the brain,
link |
01:03:19.800
which will make it easier to learn faster
link |
01:03:22.360
and easier to learn the information.
link |
01:03:24.680
So now I want to talk about auditory learning
link |
01:03:27.340
and actually how you can get better
link |
01:03:29.000
at learning information that you hear,
link |
01:03:30.960
not just information that you see on a page
link |
01:03:33.860
or motor skill learning.
link |
01:03:35.920
There are a lot of reasons to want to do this.
link |
01:03:37.560
A lot of classroom teaching,
link |
01:03:39.000
whether or not it's by Zoom or in person,
link |
01:03:41.520
is auditory in nature.
link |
01:03:42.960
Not everything is necessarily written down for us.
link |
01:03:47.080
It's also good to get better at listening, or so I'm told.
link |
01:03:52.220
So there's a phenomenon called the cocktail party effect.
link |
01:03:55.960
Now, even if you've never been to a cocktail party,
link |
01:03:58.040
you've experienced and participated
link |
01:04:00.780
in what's called the cocktail party effect.
link |
01:04:02.900
The cocktail party effect is where you are in an environment
link |
01:04:05.760
that's rich with sound,
link |
01:04:07.100
many sound waves coming from many different sources,
link |
01:04:09.760
many different things.
link |
01:04:10.600
So in a city, in a classroom,
link |
01:04:13.220
in a car that contains people having various conversations,
link |
01:04:17.500
you somehow need to be able to attend
link |
01:04:20.560
to specific components of those sound waves,
link |
01:04:22.880
meaning you need to hear certain people and not others.
link |
01:04:25.640
The reason it's called the cocktail party effect
link |
01:04:27.920
is that you, and meaning your brain,
link |
01:04:31.720
are exquisitely good at creating a cone
link |
01:04:36.140
of auditory attention, a narrow band of attention
link |
01:04:39.640
with which you can extract the information you care about
link |
01:04:42.760
and wipe away or erase all the rest.
link |
01:04:47.180
Now, this takes work.
link |
01:04:48.600
It takes attention.
link |
01:04:50.360
One of the reasons why you might come home
link |
01:04:52.380
from a loud gathering, maybe a stadium, a sports event,
link |
01:04:55.960
or a cocktail party for that matter,
link |
01:04:57.720
and feel just exhausted is because if you were listening
link |
01:05:00.560
to conversations there or trying to listen
link |
01:05:02.500
to those conversations while watching the game
link |
01:05:04.420
and people moving past you and hearing all this noise,
link |
01:05:08.000
clinking of glasses, et cetera,
link |
01:05:10.520
it takes attentional effort,
link |
01:05:12.360
and the brain uses up a lot of energy just at rest,
link |
01:05:17.520
but it uses up even more energy
link |
01:05:19.940
when you are paying strong attention to something,
link |
01:05:22.160
literally caloric energy,
link |
01:05:23.640
burning up things like glucose, et cetera.
link |
01:05:25.500
Even if you're ketogenic, it's burning up energy.
link |
01:05:28.640
So the cocktail party effect has been studied extensively
link |
01:05:32.660
in the field of neuroscience,
link |
01:05:33.840
and we now know at a mechanistic level
link |
01:05:36.580
how one accomplishes this feat of attending to certain sounds
link |
01:05:40.740
despite the fact that we are being bombarded
link |
01:05:43.480
with all sorts of other sounds.
link |
01:05:45.380
So there are a couple of ways that we do this.
link |
01:05:46.880
First of all, much as with our visual system,
link |
01:05:51.980
we can expand or contract our visual field of view.
link |
01:05:57.760
So we can go from panoramic vision,
link |
01:05:59.560
see the entire scene that we are in by dilating our gaze.
link |
01:06:03.320
I've talked a lot about this on this podcast and elsewhere.
link |
01:06:05.840
We can, for instance, keep our head and eyes stationary
link |
01:06:08.520
or mostly stationary.
link |
01:06:09.560
You don't have to be rigid about it,
link |
01:06:10.560
and you can expand your field of view
link |
01:06:11.920
so you can see the walls and ceiling and floor.
link |
01:06:14.200
You can see yourself in the environment.
link |
01:06:15.360
That's panoramic view.
link |
01:06:16.720
It's what you would accomplish without having to try at all
link |
01:06:18.880
if you went to a horizon, for instance.
link |
01:06:21.880
Or we can contract our field of view.
link |
01:06:23.520
I can bring my focus to a particular location,
link |
01:06:26.720
what we call a vergence point, directly in front of me.
link |
01:06:28.560
Now I'm pointing at the camera directly in front of me.
link |
01:06:31.480
Okay, we can do that.
link |
01:06:32.320
We can expand and contract our visual field of view.
link |
01:06:34.220
Well, we can expand and contract our auditory field of view,
link |
01:06:40.200
so to speak, or our auditory window.
link |
01:06:43.080
You can try this next time you are in an environment
link |
01:06:46.400
that's rich with noise, meaning lots of different sounds.
link |
01:06:49.440
You can just tune out all the noise to a background chatter.
link |
01:06:54.720
You kind of just, you try not focus
link |
01:06:56.900
on any one particular sound,
link |
01:06:59.280
and you get the background kind of chatter of noise.
link |
01:07:02.960
And you'll find that it's actually very relaxing
link |
01:07:05.040
in comparison to trying to listen to somebody
link |
01:07:06.840
at a cocktail party and you're shouting back and forth.
link |
01:07:08.560
Now, if you're very, very interested in that person
link |
01:07:10.560
or getting to know them better or what they're telling you
link |
01:07:13.940
or some combination of those things,
link |
01:07:15.440
then you'll be very motivated to do it,
link |
01:07:17.420
but nonetheless, it requires energy and effort and attention.
link |
01:07:22.280
How do we do this?
link |
01:07:23.280
Well, it's actually quite simple,
link |
01:07:26.120
or at least it's simple in essence,
link |
01:07:29.600
although the underlying mechanisms are complex.
link |
01:07:32.760
Here, I have to credit the laboratory
link |
01:07:35.900
of a guy named Mike Wehr, W-E-H-R,
link |
01:07:39.560
up at the University of Oregon,
link |
01:07:41.160
who essentially figured out that we are able
link |
01:07:45.220
to accomplish this extraction of particular sounds.
link |
01:07:48.020
We can really hear one person or a small number of people
link |
01:07:52.160
amidst a huge background of chatter
link |
01:07:54.840
because we pay attention to the onset of words,
link |
01:07:59.960
but also to the offset of words.
link |
01:08:03.000
Now, the way to visualize this is if the background noise
link |
01:08:07.040
is just like a bunch of waves of noise,
link |
01:08:08.920
it's literally just sound waves coming every frequency,
link |
01:08:11.320
low frequency, high frequency, glasses clinking together.
link |
01:08:13.720
If you're at a game, people are shouting,
link |
01:08:15.200
people are talking on their phone.
link |
01:08:17.080
There's the crack of the ball if somebody actually manages
link |
01:08:20.280
to hit the ball, the announcer, et cetera.
link |
01:08:24.420
But whatever we're paying attention to,
link |
01:08:27.300
we set up a cone of auditory attention,
link |
01:08:29.620
a kind of a tunnel of auditory attention
link |
01:08:32.280
where we are listening, although we don't realize it,
link |
01:08:34.400
we are listening for the onset and the offset of those words.
link |
01:08:39.060
Now, this is powerful for a couple of reasons.
link |
01:08:41.680
First of all, it's a call to arms, so to speak,
link |
01:08:47.340
to disengage your auditory system
link |
01:08:50.520
when you don't need to focus your attention
link |
01:08:52.420
on something particular.
link |
01:08:53.840
So if you are somebody, you're coming home from work,
link |
01:08:56.020
you've had a very long day,
link |
01:08:57.760
and you're trying to make out a particular conversation
link |
01:09:01.820
on background noise,
link |
01:09:03.420
you might consider just not having that conversation,
link |
01:09:06.380
just letting your auditory landscape be very broad,
link |
01:09:10.420
almost like panoramic vision.
link |
01:09:13.040
If you're trying to learn
link |
01:09:14.600
how to extract sound information,
link |
01:09:17.040
it could be notes of music, it could be scales of music,
link |
01:09:22.040
it could be words spoken by somebody else.
link |
01:09:24.780
Maybe somebody is telling you what you need to say
link |
01:09:27.500
for a particular speech,
link |
01:09:29.080
or the information that you need to learn
link |
01:09:31.440
for a particular topic, and they're telling it to you.
link |
01:09:34.920
Deliberately paying attention both to the onset
link |
01:09:37.740
and to the offset of those words can be beneficial
link |
01:09:41.320
because it is exactly the way that the auditory system
link |
01:09:44.860
likes to bring in information.
link |
01:09:47.220
So one of the more common phenomenon
link |
01:09:50.460
that I think we all experience
link |
01:09:51.740
is you go to a party, or you meet somebody new,
link |
01:09:54.940
and you say, hi, I would say, hi, I'm Andrew,
link |
01:09:57.360
and they'd say, hi, I'm Jeff, for instance,
link |
01:09:59.820
great to meet you.
link |
01:10:00.660
And then a minute later, I can't remember the guy's name.
link |
01:10:03.920
Now, is it because I don't care what his name is?
link |
01:10:05.660
No, somehow the presence
link |
01:10:08.100
of other auditory information interfered.
link |
01:10:10.260
It's not that my mind was necessarily someplace else,
link |
01:10:12.740
it's that the signal to noise, as we say, wasn't high enough.
link |
01:10:17.500
Somehow the way he said it or the way it landed on my ears,
link |
01:10:22.460
which is really all that matters, right,
link |
01:10:24.660
when it comes down to learning,
link |
01:10:26.440
is such that it just didn't achieve
link |
01:10:29.820
high enough signal to noise.
link |
01:10:31.620
The noise was too high or the signal was too low
link |
01:10:33.940
or some combination of those.
link |
01:10:35.820
So the next time you ask somebody's name,
link |
01:10:37.460
remember, listen to the onset of what they say
link |
01:10:40.060
and the offset.
link |
01:10:40.900
So it would be paying attention to the j in Jeff,
link |
01:10:43.940
and it would be paying attention to the th in F, in Jeff,
link |
01:10:47.500
excuse me, all right?
link |
01:10:49.540
And chances are you'll be able to remember that name.
link |
01:10:53.140
Now, I don't know if people who are super learners of names
link |
01:10:58.020
do this naturally or not.
link |
01:10:59.380
I don't have access to their brains.
link |
01:11:01.020
I don't think they're going to give me access
link |
01:11:02.180
to their brains either,
link |
01:11:03.500
but it's a very interesting way to take the natural biology
link |
01:11:07.060
of auditory attention and learning
link |
01:11:08.820
and apply it to scenarios where you're trying to remember
link |
01:11:11.700
either people's names or specific information.
link |
01:11:14.540
Now, I do acknowledge that trying to learn every word
link |
01:11:17.580
in a sentence by paying attention to its onset and offset
link |
01:11:21.560
could actually be kind of disruptive
link |
01:11:23.020
to the learning process.
link |
01:11:24.780
So this would be more for specific attention.
link |
01:11:27.340
Like you're asking directions in a city and somebody says,
link |
01:11:29.520
okay, you say you're lost, and they say, okay,
link |
01:11:32.220
you're going to go two blocks down,
link |
01:11:34.540
you're going to turn left,
link |
01:11:35.860
and then you're going to see a landmark on your right,
link |
01:11:37.820
and then you're going to go in the third door on your left.
link |
01:11:42.860
That's a lot of information, at least for me, okay?
link |
01:11:45.420
So the way you would want to listen to that is
link |
01:11:48.180
you're going to go down the road.
link |
01:11:50.260
See, I already forgot.
link |
01:11:51.240
You're going to go left, and you're just going to program,
link |
01:11:54.720
and instead of just hearing the word left,
link |
01:11:56.220
you're going to think the L at the front of left and the T.
link |
01:11:59.900
You're going to left, okay?
link |
01:12:02.140
And then, so you're coding in specific words,
link |
01:12:05.060
and what this does is this kind of hijacks
link |
01:12:07.780
these naturally occurring attention mechanisms
link |
01:12:09.980
that the auditory system likes to use.
link |
01:12:12.500
It's a little bit of data that for auditory encoding,
link |
01:12:15.460
this kind of thing can be beneficial.
link |
01:12:17.640
There are a lot of data that attention
link |
01:12:20.580
for auditory coding is beneficial.
link |
01:12:23.660
There are a little bit of data showing
link |
01:12:26.660
that deliberately encoding auditory information this way,
link |
01:12:29.980
meaning trying to learn auditory information this way,
link |
01:12:32.760
can be beneficial or can accelerate learning.
link |
01:12:35.580
And some of these features of what I'm describing here
link |
01:12:39.000
map onto some of the work of Mike Merzenich and others
link |
01:12:43.580
that have been designed to try and overcome things
link |
01:12:46.500
like stutter and to treat various forms
link |
01:12:49.780
of auditory learning disorders.
link |
01:12:52.180
But more importantly, and perhaps more powerful,
link |
01:12:56.620
is the work of Mike Merzenich
link |
01:12:59.020
that was done with his then graduate student,
link |
01:13:01.260
Greg Recanzone, that showed that
link |
01:13:03.700
using the attentional system,
link |
01:13:06.700
we can actually learn much faster
link |
01:13:09.360
and we can actually activate neuroplasticity
link |
01:13:12.660
in the adult brain, something that's very challenging to do,
link |
01:13:15.980
and that the auditory system is one of the main ways
link |
01:13:19.460
in which we can access neuroplasticity more broadly.
link |
01:13:22.580
So I just want to take a couple of minutes
link |
01:13:24.120
and describe the work of Recanzone and Merzenich
link |
01:13:26.340
because it's absolutely fantastic and fascinating.
link |
01:13:30.500
What they did is they had subjects
link |
01:13:33.980
try to learn auditory information,
link |
01:13:36.940
except that they told them to pay attention
link |
01:13:40.020
to particular frequencies.
link |
01:13:42.400
So now you know what frequencies are,
link |
01:13:43.760
so essentially high-pitched sounds or low-pitched sounds.
link |
01:13:48.620
What they found was just passively listening
link |
01:13:52.300
to a bunch of stuff does not allow the brain to change
link |
01:13:56.180
and for that stuff to be remembered at all.
link |
01:13:58.920
That's not a surprise.
link |
01:13:59.940
We've all experienced the phenomenon of having someone talk
link |
01:14:03.540
and we see their mouth moving and we're like,
link |
01:14:04.880
yeah, this is really important, this is really important,
link |
01:14:06.620
we're listening, we're trying to listen,
link |
01:14:07.980
and then they walk away and we think,
link |
01:14:09.260
I didn't get any of that.
link |
01:14:11.340
And you wonder whether or not it was them,
link |
01:14:13.340
maybe this is happening to you right now,
link |
01:14:14.980
you wonder whether or not it was you,
link |
01:14:17.720
you wonder whether or not you have trouble with learning
link |
01:14:19.740
or you have attention deficit,
link |
01:14:21.220
it could be any number of different things.
link |
01:14:23.180
But what Recanzone and Merzenich discovered
link |
01:14:25.260
was that if you instruct subjects to listen
link |
01:14:28.480
for particular cues within speech or within sounds,
link |
01:14:32.780
that not only can you learn those things more quickly,
link |
01:14:35.740
but that you can remap these tonotopic maps in the cortex
link |
01:14:42.080
that I referred to earlier,
link |
01:14:43.140
you actually get changes in the neural architecture,
link |
01:14:45.420
the neural circuitry in the brain,
link |
01:14:46.980
and this can occur not only very rapidly,
link |
01:14:50.640
but they can occur in the adult brain,
link |
01:14:53.100
which prior to their work
link |
01:14:54.860
was not thought to be amenable to change.
link |
01:14:57.340
It was long thought that neuroplasticity
link |
01:14:59.420
could only occur in the developing brain,
link |
01:15:01.220
but the work of Recanzone and Merzenich
link |
01:15:03.180
in the auditory system actually was some of the first
link |
01:15:06.780
that really opened up everybody's eyes and ears
link |
01:15:10.700
to the idea that the brain can change in adulthood.
link |
01:15:14.060
So here's how this sort of process would work
link |
01:15:16.340
and how you might apply it.
link |
01:15:18.340
If you are trying to learn music
link |
01:15:20.540
or you're trying to learn information
link |
01:15:23.520
that you're going to then recite,
link |
01:15:25.180
you can decide to highlight certain words
link |
01:15:30.100
or certain frequencies of sound
link |
01:15:33.620
or certain scales or certain keys on the piano
link |
01:15:36.380
and to only focus on those for certain learning bouts, okay?
link |
01:15:40.220
So I'll give an example that's sort of real time for me,
link |
01:15:44.320
meaning it's happening right now.
link |
01:15:47.860
I know generally what I want to say when I arrive here.
link |
01:15:51.340
I even know specifically certain things
link |
01:15:53.280
that I want to make sure get across to you,
link |
01:15:56.380
but I don't think about every single word
link |
01:15:58.900
that I'm going to say and the precise order
link |
01:16:01.460
in which I'm going to say those things.
link |
01:16:03.300
That would be actually very disruptive
link |
01:16:05.200
because it wouldn't match my normal patterns of speech
link |
01:16:07.500
and you'd probably think I was sounding rather robotic
link |
01:16:10.540
if I were to do that.
link |
01:16:13.200
So one way that we can remember information is
link |
01:16:15.820
as we write out, for instance, something that we want to say,
link |
01:16:19.140
we can highlight particular words.
link |
01:16:20.900
We can underline those.
link |
01:16:22.260
If we're listening to somebody
link |
01:16:25.520
and they are telling us information,
link |
01:16:28.760
we can decide just to highlight particular words
link |
01:16:31.740
that they said to us and write those down.
link |
01:16:34.400
Now, of course, we're listening to all the information,
link |
01:16:36.980
but the work of Reckinzone and Murzenich
link |
01:16:39.860
and the work of others, in addition to them,
link |
01:16:42.480
his former student or former postdoc, I don't know which,
link |
01:16:46.620
Michael Kilgard, who's now got his own lab down in Texas,
link |
01:16:49.540
or others, have shown that the queuing of attention
link |
01:16:52.440
to particular features of speech,
link |
01:16:54.700
particular components of speech,
link |
01:16:57.020
the way in which it increases our level of attention overall
link |
01:17:00.440
allows us to capture more of the information overall.
link |
01:17:04.120
And so I don't want this to be abstract at all.
link |
01:17:05.960
What this means is when you're listening,
link |
01:17:07.880
you don't have to listen to every word.
link |
01:17:10.400
You're already listening to every word.
link |
01:17:12.080
All the information's coming in through your ears.
link |
01:17:14.380
What you're trying to extract is particular things
link |
01:17:18.760
or themes within the content.
link |
01:17:20.720
So maybe you decide, if you're listening to me,
link |
01:17:22.580
that you're only going to listen to the word tools,
link |
01:17:26.160
or you're only going to listen
link |
01:17:27.200
to when my voice kind of goes above background.
link |
01:17:29.800
You get to decide what you decide to listen to or not.
link |
01:17:32.680
And what you decide to focus on
link |
01:17:34.960
isn't necessarily as important
link |
01:17:37.360
as the fact that you're focusing.
link |
01:17:39.240
So I hope that's clear.
link |
01:17:40.520
The auditory system does this all the time
link |
01:17:42.800
with the cocktail party effect.
link |
01:17:44.000
What I'm talking about is exporting certain elements
link |
01:17:46.720
of the mechanisms of the cocktail party effect,
link |
01:17:49.360
paying attention to the onset and offset of words,
link |
01:17:51.800
or particular notes within music, or particular scales,
link |
01:17:55.520
or you can make it even broader
link |
01:17:58.020
and particular motifs of music,
link |
01:18:00.200
or particular sentences of words, or particular phrases.
link |
01:18:03.520
And in doing that,
link |
01:18:04.480
you extract more of the information overall,
link |
01:18:08.280
even though you're not paying attention
link |
01:18:09.480
to all the information at once.
link |
01:18:11.760
Now I'd like to talk about a phenomenon
link |
01:18:13.440
that you've all experienced before,
link |
01:18:15.900
which is called Doppler.
link |
01:18:17.920
So the Doppler effect is the way that we experience sound
link |
01:18:24.300
when the thing that's making that sound is moving.
link |
01:18:28.880
The simplest way to explain this
link |
01:18:30.520
is to translate the sound into the visual world once again.
link |
01:18:35.360
So if you've ever seen a duck or a goose
link |
01:18:39.080
sitting in a pond or a lake,
link |
01:18:41.800
and it's kind of bobbing up and down,
link |
01:18:44.420
what you'll notice is that the ripples of water
link |
01:18:46.580
that extend out from that duck or goose
link |
01:18:49.360
are fairly regularly spaced in all directions.
link |
01:18:52.080
And that's because that duck or goose is stationary.
link |
01:18:54.720
It's moving up and down,
link |
01:18:55.540
but it's not moving forward or backward or to the side.
link |
01:18:58.960
Now, if that duck or goose were to swim forward
link |
01:19:02.080
by paddling its little webbed feet under the surface,
link |
01:19:05.920
you would immediately notice that the ripples of water
link |
01:19:09.080
that are close to and in front of that duck or goose
link |
01:19:12.460
would be closer together than the ones that trailed it
link |
01:19:15.480
that were behind.
link |
01:19:16.920
And that is essentially what happens with sound as well.
link |
01:19:21.160
With the Doppler effect,
link |
01:19:23.320
we experience sounds that are closer to us
link |
01:19:25.720
at higher frequency, the ripples are closer together,
link |
01:19:29.440
and sounds that are further away at lower frequency,
link |
01:19:32.720
especially when they're moving past us.
link |
01:19:35.000
So if you've ever, for instance,
link |
01:19:37.060
heard a siren in the distance,
link |
01:19:39.300
nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh, nuh,
link |
01:19:47.760
that's essentially my rendition of a siren,
link |
01:19:50.400
I don't know what ambulance or police or what,
link |
01:19:52.840
passing you on a street.
link |
01:19:54.920
That is the Doppler effect.
link |
01:19:57.280
The Doppler effect is one of the main ways
link |
01:19:59.920
that we make out the direction that things are arriving from
link |
01:20:05.440
and their speeds and trajectories.
link |
01:20:08.040
And we get very good from a very young age
link |
01:20:11.680
at discerning what direction things are arriving from
link |
01:20:15.280
and the direction that they are going to pass us in.
link |
01:20:19.320
And the Doppler effect has probably saved your life
link |
01:20:21.240
many, many times.
link |
01:20:23.000
In this way, you just don't realize it
link |
01:20:24.520
because you'll step off the curb
link |
01:20:25.920
or you're driving your car and you pull to the side
link |
01:20:29.280
so that the ambulance or fire truck can go by
link |
01:20:32.120
because you heard that siren off in the distance.
link |
01:20:37.040
And then you pull away from the curb
link |
01:20:38.500
and you get back on the road,
link |
01:20:39.960
in part because you don't see it any longer,
link |
01:20:41.840
but also you don't hear any other sirens in the distance.
link |
01:20:44.940
Now, some animals such as bats are exquisitely good
link |
01:20:49.000
at navigating their environments according to sound.
link |
01:20:51.920
Now, we've all heard that bats don't see,
link |
01:20:55.320
that's actually not true, they actually have vision,
link |
01:20:57.680
but they just rely more heavily on their auditory system.
link |
01:21:00.280
And the way that bats navigate in the dark
link |
01:21:03.480
and the way that bats navigate using sound
link |
01:21:06.280
is through Doppler.
link |
01:21:07.880
Now, they don't simply listen to whether or not
link |
01:21:11.120
things are coming at them or moving away from them
link |
01:21:14.640
and pay attention to Doppler
link |
01:21:15.760
like the siren example I gave for you.
link |
01:21:18.920
What they do is they generate their own sounds.
link |
01:21:22.600
So a bat, as it flies around, is sending out clicks.
link |
01:21:29.080
I think that's my best bat sound, or maybe it is.
link |
01:21:32.320
And they're clicking, they're actually propelling sound out
link |
01:21:35.080
at a particular frequency that they know.
link |
01:21:38.320
Now, whether or not they're conscious of it, I don't know.
link |
01:21:40.180
I've never asked them.
link |
01:21:41.020
And if I did ask them, I don't think they could answer.
link |
01:21:42.500
And if they could answer,
link |
01:21:43.340
they couldn't answer in a language that I could understand.
link |
01:21:45.820
But the bat is essentially flying around,
link |
01:21:48.120
sending out sound waves,
link |
01:21:49.760
pinging its environment with sound waves
link |
01:21:52.040
of a particular frequency.
link |
01:21:53.320
And then depending on the frequency of sound waves
link |
01:21:55.800
that come back, they know if they're getting closer
link |
01:21:58.900
to an object or further away from it.
link |
01:22:01.120
So if they send out sounds at a frequency of,
link |
01:22:03.880
this was much slower than it would actually occur,
link |
01:22:06.660
but let's say one every half second,
link |
01:22:09.560
and it's coming back even faster,
link |
01:22:13.780
then they know they're getting closer
link |
01:22:15.700
because of the Doppler effect.
link |
01:22:18.000
And if it comes back more slowly,
link |
01:22:20.240
they know that there's nothing in front of them.
link |
01:22:22.260
So the bat is essentially navigating its world
link |
01:22:25.820
by creating these auras of sound
link |
01:22:29.380
that bounce back onto them from the various objects,
link |
01:22:32.940
trees, et cetera, buildings, and people.
link |
01:22:35.500
It's kind of eerie to think about,
link |
01:22:36.380
but yes, they see you, they experience you with their sound,
link |
01:22:39.900
they sense you, and they're using Doppler to accomplish it.
link |
01:22:44.420
Now I'd like to talk about ringing in the ears.
link |
01:22:47.980
This is something that I get asked about a lot.
link |
01:22:50.980
And speaking of signal to noise,
link |
01:22:53.340
I don't know if I get asked about it a lot
link |
01:22:55.540
because many people suffer from ringing in their ears
link |
01:22:58.900
or because the people who suffer from ringing in their ears
link |
01:23:02.100
suffer so much that they are more prone to ask.
link |
01:23:05.460
So it could be a sampling bias, I don't know,
link |
01:23:08.300
but I've been asked enough times,
link |
01:23:10.420
and some of the experiences of discomfort
link |
01:23:12.780
that people have expressed
link |
01:23:14.340
about having this ringing of the ears
link |
01:23:17.300
really motivate me to go deep into this literature.
link |
01:23:20.660
So the ringing of the ears that one experiences
link |
01:23:24.100
is called tinnitus, not tinnitus, but tinnitus.
link |
01:23:29.100
And tinnitus can vary in intensity
link |
01:23:32.700
and it can vary according to stress levels.
link |
01:23:36.220
It can vary across the lifespan or even time of day.
link |
01:23:40.500
So it's very subject to kind of background effects
link |
01:23:43.360
and contextual effects.
link |
01:23:45.800
So I think we all know that we should do our best
link |
01:23:48.820
to maximize healthy sleep.
link |
01:23:50.180
We did a number of episodes on that.
link |
01:23:52.020
Essentially the first four episodes
link |
01:23:53.780
of the Huberman Lab Podcast were all about sleep
link |
01:23:55.620
and how to get better sleep.
link |
01:23:57.020
We all know that we should try and limit our stress,
link |
01:24:00.120
and we had an episode about stress
link |
01:24:02.220
and ways to mitigate stress as well.
link |
01:24:05.960
However, there are people, it seems,
link |
01:24:08.440
that are suffering from tinnitus
link |
01:24:09.860
for which stress or lack of sleep
link |
01:24:12.400
just can't explain the presence of the tinnitus.
link |
01:24:15.900
Tinnitus can be caused by disruption to these hair cells
link |
01:24:20.580
that we talked about earlier or damage to the hair cells.
link |
01:24:23.660
So that's another reason why,
link |
01:24:25.180
even if you have good hearing now,
link |
01:24:28.420
that you want to protect that hearing
link |
01:24:30.060
and really avoid putting yourself
link |
01:24:32.540
into these kind of two-hit environments,
link |
01:24:34.660
environments where there's a lot of background noise
link |
01:24:36.920
and then you add another really loud auditory stimulus.
link |
01:24:40.940
This also can happen at different times, I should mention.
link |
01:24:43.540
If you go to a concert or you listen to loud music
link |
01:24:46.860
with your headphones and then you go to a concert
link |
01:24:49.620
or you go into a very loud work environment,
link |
01:24:52.140
the hair cells can still be vulnerable.
link |
01:24:53.900
And once those hair cells are knocked out,
link |
01:24:56.560
currently we don't have the technology to put them back,
link |
01:24:58.780
although many groups,
link |
01:24:59.940
including some excellent groups at Stanford and elsewhere,
link |
01:25:03.220
too, of course, are working on ways
link |
01:25:05.380
to replenish those hair cells and restore hearing.
link |
01:25:09.800
There are treatments for tinnitus
link |
01:25:11.540
that involve taking certain substances.
link |
01:25:17.340
There are medications for tinnitus.
link |
01:25:19.860
In the non-prescription landscape,
link |
01:25:21.820
which is typically what we discuss on this podcast
link |
01:25:24.300
when we discuss taking anything,
link |
01:25:27.620
there are essentially four compounds
link |
01:25:29.620
for which there are quality peer-reviewed data
link |
01:25:32.580
where there does not appear to be any overt commercial bias,
link |
01:25:36.880
meaning that nothing's reported in the papers
link |
01:25:39.060
as funding from a particular company.
link |
01:25:41.140
And those are melatonin, ginkgo bilboa, zinc, and magnesium.
link |
01:25:46.420
Now, I've talked about melatonin before.
link |
01:25:49.060
I'm personally not a fan of melatonin as a sleep aid,
link |
01:25:53.100
but there are four studies.
link |
01:25:56.380
First one entitled,
link |
01:25:57.220
the effects of melatonin on tinnitus and sleep.
link |
01:26:02.660
Second one, treatment of central and sensory neural tinnitus
link |
01:26:05.780
with orally administered melatonin.
link |
01:26:07.620
And then the title goes on much longer,
link |
01:26:09.620
but it's a randomized study.
link |
01:26:11.560
I'm not going to read out all of these.
link |
01:26:12.700
Melatonin, can it stop the ringing?
link |
01:26:14.660
Which is an interesting article, double-blinded study.
link |
01:26:17.700
And the effects of melatonin on tinnitus.
link |
01:26:21.020
Each one of these studies has anywhere from 30
link |
01:26:23.820
to more than 100 subjects, in one case, 102 subjects,
link |
01:26:28.260
both genders as they list them out.
link |
01:26:30.980
Typically it's listed as sex, not gender in studies.
link |
01:26:34.060
So it should say both sexes, but nonetheless.
link |
01:26:38.780
An age range anywhere from 30 years old
link |
01:26:42.580
all the way up to 65 plus.
link |
01:26:44.380
I didn't see any studies of people younger than 30.
link |
01:26:48.760
All three focused on melatonin,
link |
01:26:51.980
not surprisingly because of the titles.
link |
01:26:54.620
Looking at a range of dosages
link |
01:26:56.060
anywhere from three milligrams per day,
link |
01:26:58.620
which is sort of typical of many supplements for melatonin,
link |
01:27:02.280
still much higher than one would manufacture
link |
01:27:05.720
endogenously through your own pineal gland.
link |
01:27:08.300
But three milligrams in these studies
link |
01:27:12.300
for a duration of anywhere from 30 days
link |
01:27:15.340
to much longer in some cases, six months.
link |
01:27:19.260
And all four of these studies found modest,
link |
01:27:22.680
yet still statistically significant effects
link |
01:27:26.040
of taking melatonin by mouth,
link |
01:27:28.200
so it's orally administered melatonin,
link |
01:27:31.340
in reducing the severity of tinnitus.
link |
01:27:36.300
So that's compelling, at least to me.
link |
01:27:39.060
It doesn't sound like a cure.
link |
01:27:40.940
And of course, as always, I'm not a physician.
link |
01:27:44.420
I'm a scientist, so I don't prescribe anything.
link |
01:27:46.460
I only profess things.
link |
01:27:47.480
I report to you the science.
link |
01:27:48.640
You have to decide if melatonin is right for you,
link |
01:27:51.060
if you have tinnitus.
link |
01:27:53.100
And certainly I say that both to protect myself,
link |
01:27:56.260
but also protect you.
link |
01:27:57.560
You're responsible for your health and wellbeing.
link |
01:28:00.000
And I'm not telling anyone to run out
link |
01:28:02.340
and start taking melatonin for tinnitus,
link |
01:28:05.240
but it does seem that it can have some effects
link |
01:28:07.300
in reducing its symptoms.
link |
01:28:09.480
Ginkgo boa boa is an interesting compound.
link |
01:28:13.080
It's been prescribed for or recommended for many,
link |
01:28:17.160
many things, but there are a few studies,
link |
01:28:21.840
again, double-blinded studies lasting one to six months.
link |
01:28:24.920
Any one that has an impressive number of subjects,
link |
01:28:27.940
978 subjects ranging from age 18 all the way up to 65,
link |
01:28:33.080
so on and so forth,
link |
01:28:34.900
that show not huge effects of ginkgo,
link |
01:28:40.440
but as they quote,
link |
01:28:41.840
"'limited evidence' suggests that if tinnitus
link |
01:28:44.520
is a side effect of something else,
link |
01:28:47.160
in particular cognitive decline,"
link |
01:28:49.440
so age-related tinnitus might be helped by ginkgo boa boa.
link |
01:28:56.800
I won't go through all the details of the zinc studies,
link |
01:28:58.960
but it seems that zinc supplementation at higher levels
link |
01:29:01.420
than are typical of most people's intake,
link |
01:29:03.280
so 50 milligrams per day,
link |
01:29:05.280
do appear to be able to reduce subjective symptoms
link |
01:29:07.460
of tinnitus in most of the people
link |
01:29:09.940
that took the supplemented zinc.
link |
01:29:11.720
There aren't a lot of studies on that,
link |
01:29:13.860
so I could only find one double-blinded study.
link |
01:29:16.800
It lasted anywhere from one to six months, 41 subjects,
link |
01:29:19.560
both genders listed out again here, 45 to 64,
link |
01:29:23.400
and they saw a decrease in the severity
link |
01:29:25.040
of tinnitus symptoms with 50 milligrams
link |
01:29:28.880
of elemental zinc supplementation.
link |
01:29:31.280
And then last but not least is the magnesium study.
link |
01:29:34.320
Again, only a single study.
link |
01:29:36.360
It's a phase II study
link |
01:29:38.320
looking at a fairly limited number of subjects,
link |
01:29:41.000
so only 19 subjects,
link |
01:29:42.920
taking 532 milligrams of elemental magnesium.
link |
01:29:46.440
For those of you that take magnesium,
link |
01:29:47.720
there's magnesium and elemental magnesium,
link |
01:29:49.560
and it's always translated on the bottle,
link |
01:29:52.280
but it was associated with a lessening of symptoms
link |
01:29:54.760
related to tinnitus.
link |
01:29:56.060
So for you tinnitus sufferers out there,
link |
01:29:59.720
you may already be aware of this.
link |
01:30:01.200
You may already be taking these things
link |
01:30:03.660
and had no positive effects,
link |
01:30:06.280
meaning they didn't help, maybe not.
link |
01:30:08.440
I hope that you'll at least consider these,
link |
01:30:11.520
talk to your doctor about them.
link |
01:30:13.120
I do realize that tinnitus is extremely disruptive.
link |
01:30:16.240
I can't say I empathize
link |
01:30:18.880
because I don't from a place of experience, that is,
link |
01:30:21.520
because I don't have tinnitus,
link |
01:30:22.660
but for those of you that don't, including myself,
link |
01:30:24.640
you can imagine that hearing sounds
link |
01:30:27.120
of things that aren't there
link |
01:30:28.320
and the ringing in one's ears can be very disruptive,
link |
01:30:30.400
and I think would be very disruptive,
link |
01:30:33.000
and it explains why people with tinnitus reach out so often
link |
01:30:36.300
with questions about how to alleviate that,
link |
01:30:38.320
and I hope this information was useful to you.
link |
01:30:40.840
I'd like to now talk about balance and our sense of balance,
link |
01:30:44.800
which is controlled by, believe it or not,
link |
01:30:48.040
our ears and things in our ears,
link |
01:30:50.960
as well as by our brain and elements of our spinal cord.
link |
01:30:55.540
But before I do that, I want to ask you another question,
link |
01:30:58.160
or I would rather, I'd like to ask you
link |
01:31:00.720
to ask yourself a question and answer it,
link |
01:31:03.360
which is how big are your ears?
link |
01:31:07.240
It turns out that the ears grow our entire life.
link |
01:31:11.960
In the early stage of our life, they grow more slowly,
link |
01:31:15.400
and then as we age, they grow more quickly.
link |
01:31:19.120
You may have noticed if you have family members
link |
01:31:22.120
who are well into their 70s and 80s,
link |
01:31:25.000
and if you're fortunate into their 90s
link |
01:31:28.840
and maybe even hundreds is that the ears
link |
01:31:32.320
of some of these individuals get very, very big
link |
01:31:35.160
relative to their previous ear sizes.
link |
01:31:38.400
It turns out that biological age
link |
01:31:41.440
can actually be measured according to ear size.
link |
01:31:46.180
Now, you have to take a few measurements,
link |
01:31:47.500
but believe it or not, there is a formula
link |
01:31:50.400
in the scientific literature.
link |
01:31:51.880
If you know your ear circumference,
link |
01:31:56.560
so the distance around your ear, ears, plural,
link |
01:32:01.320
presumably you have two, most people do, in millimeters,
link |
01:32:05.280
so you're going to take the circumference of your ears
link |
01:32:07.980
in millimeters, how would you do this, right?
link |
01:32:10.120
How would you do this?
link |
01:32:11.040
Maybe you take a string and you put it around your ear
link |
01:32:14.360
and then you measure the string.
link |
01:32:15.400
That's probably going to be easier
link |
01:32:16.400
than marching around your ear or somebody else's ear
link |
01:32:19.560
with a ruler and measuring in millimeters.
link |
01:32:21.560
So what's your ear circumference?
link |
01:32:23.520
On the outside, don't go in on the divot or anything,
link |
01:32:26.380
you're just going around as if you were going to trace
link |
01:32:28.020
the closest fitting oval, assuming your ears are oval,
link |
01:32:32.320
closest fitting oval that matches your ear circumference.
link |
01:32:36.980
Take that number in millimeters, subtract from it.
link |
01:32:40.920
Oh, excuse me, I should do this correctly.
link |
01:32:43.240
Do that for both ears, add them together,
link |
01:32:46.040
add those numbers together, divide by two,
link |
01:32:47.840
get the average for your two ears,
link |
01:32:50.120
get your average ear circumference
link |
01:32:51.800
from across your two ears,
link |
01:32:53.520
then take that number in millimeters, subtract 88.1,
link |
01:32:59.600
and then whatever value that is, multiply it times 1.96,
link |
01:33:04.360
and that will tell you your biological age.
link |
01:33:07.360
Now, why in the world would this be accurate?
link |
01:33:10.700
Well, as we age, there are changes in a number
link |
01:33:14.080
of different biological pathways.
link |
01:33:17.620
One of those pathways is the pathways related
link |
01:33:19.680
to collagen synthesis.
link |
01:33:21.760
So not only are our ears growing,
link |
01:33:24.520
but our noses are growing too.
link |
01:33:27.000
My nose seems to be growing a lot, but then again,
link |
01:33:29.380
I did sports where I would get my nose broken,
link |
01:33:31.200
something I don't recommend.
link |
01:33:32.460
So I always point out, you don't get a nose like mine
link |
01:33:34.180
doing yoga, but nonetheless, my nose is still growing
link |
01:33:38.400
and my ears are still growing, and I suspect as I get older,
link |
01:33:41.180
if I have the good fortune of living into my 80s and 90s,
link |
01:33:45.000
my ears are going to continue to grow.
link |
01:33:47.320
A comparison between chronological age
link |
01:33:49.240
and biological age is something that's
link |
01:33:50.760
of really deep interest these days,
link |
01:33:52.320
and the work of David Sinclair
link |
01:33:54.240
at Harvard Medical School and others,
link |
01:33:57.480
so-called Horvath clocks that people have developed,
link |
01:34:01.000
have tapped into how the epigenome and the genome
link |
01:34:04.200
can give us some insight into our biological age
link |
01:34:07.800
and how that compares to our chronological age.
link |
01:34:09.800
Most of us know our chronological age
link |
01:34:11.980
because we know when we were born,
link |
01:34:13.840
and we know where we are relative to that now.
link |
01:34:16.920
But you can start to make a little chart, if you like,
link |
01:34:22.160
about your rates of ear growth.
link |
01:34:23.860
Your rates of ear growth actually correlate pretty well
link |
01:34:26.320
with your rates of biological progression
link |
01:34:29.640
through this thing that we call life.
link |
01:34:32.440
So it's not something that we think about too often,
link |
01:34:34.680
but just like our DNA and our epigenome
link |
01:34:38.760
and some other markers of metabolic health
link |
01:34:41.440
and hormone health relate to our age,
link |
01:34:43.920
so does our collagen synthesis,
link |
01:34:46.000
and one of the places that shows up the most
link |
01:34:48.040
is in these two little goodies on the sides of our heads,
link |
01:34:50.240
which are our ears.
link |
01:34:51.720
So even though it's a little bit of a bizarre metric,
link |
01:34:54.880
it makes perfect sense in the biological context.
link |
01:34:57.640
So let's talk about balance
link |
01:34:59.200
and how to get better at balancing.
link |
01:35:01.680
The reason why we're talking about balance
link |
01:35:03.300
and how to get better at balancing
link |
01:35:04.960
in the episode about hearing is that all the goodies
link |
01:35:08.880
that are going to allow you to do that are in your ears.
link |
01:35:12.560
And they're also in your brain,
link |
01:35:15.300
but they're mostly in your ears.
link |
01:35:17.680
So as you recall from the beginning of this episode,
link |
01:35:19.920
you have two cochlea, cochleas,
link |
01:35:22.760
that are one on each side of your head,
link |
01:35:25.600
and that's a little spiral snail-shaped thing
link |
01:35:27.760
that converts sound waves into electrical signals
link |
01:35:30.640
that the rest of your brain can understand.
link |
01:35:33.360
Right next to those,
link |
01:35:34.680
you have what are called semicircular canals.
link |
01:35:38.920
The semicircular canals can be best visualized
link |
01:35:41.420
as thinking about three hula hoops with marbles in them.
link |
01:35:45.340
So imagine that you have a hula hoop
link |
01:35:48.320
and it's not filled with marbles all the way around,
link |
01:35:51.400
it's just got some marbles down there at the base.
link |
01:35:54.520
Okay, so if you were to move that hula hoop around,
link |
01:35:57.280
the marbles will move around.
link |
01:35:58.320
Shoo, shoo, shoo, okay?
link |
01:36:00.860
You've got three of those,
link |
01:36:01.960
and each one of those hula hoops has these marbles
link |
01:36:04.760
that can move around.
link |
01:36:05.820
One of those hula hoops is positioned vertically
link |
01:36:10.780
with respect to gravity.
link |
01:36:12.760
So it's basically parallel to your nose.
link |
01:36:15.540
It's like this, if you're watching on a video,
link |
01:36:17.520
but basically it's upright.
link |
01:36:19.160
Another one of those hula hoops
link |
01:36:21.440
is basically at a 90-degree angle to your nose.
link |
01:36:27.340
It's basically parallel to the floor
link |
01:36:29.740
if you're standing upright now, if you're seated, okay?
link |
01:36:33.660
And the other one is kind of tilted
link |
01:36:37.200
about 45 degrees in between those.
link |
01:36:40.140
Now, why is the system there?
link |
01:36:42.860
Well, those marbles within each one of those hula hoops
link |
01:36:45.260
can move around, but they'll only move around
link |
01:36:49.740
if your head moves in a particular way.
link |
01:36:51.660
And there are three planes or three ways
link |
01:36:54.520
that your head can move.
link |
01:36:56.300
Your head can move up and down, like I'm nodding right now.
link |
01:37:00.080
So that's called pitch.
link |
01:37:01.620
It's pitching forward or pitching back, okay?
link |
01:37:05.700
So it's a nod, up and down.
link |
01:37:08.480
Or I can shake my head no, side to side.
link |
01:37:11.540
That's called yaw.
link |
01:37:12.900
You pilots will be very familiar with this, yaw.
link |
01:37:15.980
Not yawn, yaw.
link |
01:37:18.260
And then there's roll, tilting the head from side to side,
link |
01:37:22.220
the way that a cute puppy might look at you
link |
01:37:24.780
from side to side, okay?
link |
01:37:26.600
Where if somebody doesn't really understand
link |
01:37:29.500
or believe what you're saying,
link |
01:37:30.660
they might tilt their head, very common phenomenon.
link |
01:37:34.220
I mean, nobody does that to me,
link |
01:37:35.500
but they do that to each other.
link |
01:37:37.640
So pitch, yaw, and roll are the movements of the head
link |
01:37:41.920
in each of the three major planes of motion, as we say.
link |
01:37:45.460
And each one of those causes those marbles to move
link |
01:37:49.720
in one or two of the various hula hoops, okay?
link |
01:37:54.460
So if I move my head up and down when I nod,
link |
01:37:57.220
one of those hula hoops, literally right now,
link |
01:38:00.040
the marbles are moving back and forth.
link |
01:38:01.380
They aren't actually marbles, by the way.
link |
01:38:03.180
These are little, kind of like little stones, basically,
link |
01:38:08.300
little calcium-like deposits.
link |
01:38:10.980
And when they roll back and forth,
link |
01:38:13.780
they deflect little hairs, little hair cells
link |
01:38:17.740
that aren't like the hair cells
link |
01:38:18.900
that we use for measuring sound waves.
link |
01:38:21.000
They're not too different, but they are different from them.
link |
01:38:24.540
Not like the hairs on our heads,
link |
01:38:27.020
but they're basically rolling past these little hair cells
link |
01:38:30.900
and causing them to deflect.
link |
01:38:32.560
And when they deflect downward, the neurons,
link |
01:38:36.140
because hair cells are neurons,
link |
01:38:37.300
send information up to the brain.
link |
01:38:40.700
So if I move my head like this,
link |
01:38:42.460
there's a physical movement of these little stones
link |
01:38:45.540
in this hula hoop, as I'm referring to it,
link |
01:38:48.380
but they deflect these hairs, send those hairs,
link |
01:38:51.700
which are neurons, those hair cells,
link |
01:38:53.420
send information off to the brain.
link |
01:38:54.900
If I move my head from side to side,
link |
01:38:56.620
different little stones move.
link |
01:38:58.580
If I roll my head, different stones move.
link |
01:39:00.860
This is an exquisite system that exists
link |
01:39:03.860
in all animals that have a jaw.
link |
01:39:07.500
So any fish that has a jaw has this system.
link |
01:39:10.580
A puppy has this system.
link |
01:39:12.260
Any animal that has a jaw has this so-called balance system,
link |
01:39:16.080
which we call the vestibular system.
link |
01:39:18.240
One of the more important things to know
link |
01:39:20.080
about the vestibular, the balance system,
link |
01:39:22.460
is that it works together with the visual system.
link |
01:39:28.060
Let's say I hear something off to my left
link |
01:39:30.020
and I swing my head over to the left to see what it is.
link |
01:39:34.020
There are two sources of information
link |
01:39:35.700
about where my head is relative to my body,
link |
01:39:38.060
and I need to know that.
link |
01:39:39.940
First of all, when I quickly move my head to the side,
link |
01:39:43.900
those little stones, as I'm referring to them,
link |
01:39:46.380
I realize they're not actually stones,
link |
01:39:47.740
but as I'm referring to them,
link |
01:39:49.420
they quickly activate those hair cells
link |
01:39:52.380
in that one semicircular canal
link |
01:39:54.880
and send a signal off to my brain
link |
01:39:56.860
that my head just moved to the side like this.
link |
01:39:58.720
Not that it went like this and pitched back,
link |
01:40:00.640
or not that it tilted, but it just moved to the side.
link |
01:40:03.940
But also, visual information slid past my field of view.
link |
01:40:08.700
I didn't have to think about it,
link |
01:40:09.720
but just slid past my field of view.
link |
01:40:11.660
And when those two signals combine,
link |
01:40:15.660
my eyes then lock to a particular location.
link |
01:40:18.580
Now, if this is at all complicated,
link |
01:40:21.220
you can actually uncouple these things.
link |
01:40:23.780
It's very easy to do.
link |
01:40:25.060
You can do this right now.
link |
01:40:26.180
In fact, I'd like you to do this experiment
link |
01:40:27.860
if you're not already doing something else
link |
01:40:29.580
that requires your attention,
link |
01:40:30.780
and certainly don't do this if you're driving.
link |
01:40:33.660
You're going to sit down
link |
01:40:35.460
and you're going to move your head to the left very slowly
link |
01:40:40.540
with your eyes open.
link |
01:40:42.060
So you're going to move it very, very slowly.
link |
01:40:45.340
The whole thing should take about five, six,
link |
01:40:47.420
maybe even 10 seconds to complete.
link |
01:40:52.100
Okay, I just did it.
link |
01:40:53.700
Now I'm going to do it very quickly.
link |
01:40:54.940
I'd like you to do it very quickly as well.
link |
01:40:58.320
Now do it slowly again.
link |
01:41:03.940
Okay, what you probably noticed
link |
01:41:05.740
is that it's very uncomfortable to do it slowly,
link |
01:41:09.380
but you can do it very quickly
link |
01:41:12.020
without much discomfort at all.
link |
01:41:13.340
You just move your head to the side.
link |
01:41:14.940
The reason is when you move your head very slowly,
link |
01:41:18.780
those little stones at the base of that hula hoop,
link |
01:41:21.980
they don't get enough momentum to move.
link |
01:41:24.000
So you're actually not generating this signal to the brain
link |
01:41:28.180
that your head is moving.
link |
01:41:29.200
And what you'll notice is that your eyes have to go boom,
link |
01:41:32.500
boom, boom, jumping over step by step.
link |
01:41:35.300
Whereas if you move your head really quickly,
link |
01:41:37.260
the signal gets off to your brain and your eyes just go,
link |
01:41:39.220
boom, right to the location you want to look at.
link |
01:41:41.860
So moving your body slowly is actually very disruptive
link |
01:41:46.280
to the vestibular system.
link |
01:41:48.940
And it's very disruptive to your visual system.
link |
01:41:52.280
Now, if you've ever had the misfortune of being on a boat
link |
01:41:54.580
and you're going through big oscillations on the boat,
link |
01:41:57.040
for those of you folks that get seasick,
link |
01:41:59.660
this can actually make certain people seasick
link |
01:42:01.460
just to hear about it.
link |
01:42:02.760
That was big oscillations going up and down
link |
01:42:05.680
and up and down.
link |
01:42:07.000
Those are very disruptive.
link |
01:42:08.740
We'll talk about nausea in a minute
link |
01:42:09.980
and how to offset that kind of nausea.
link |
01:42:12.060
I get pretty seasick,
link |
01:42:12.980
but there are ways that you can deal with this.
link |
01:42:16.140
But this is incredible because what it means
link |
01:42:18.180
is it's a purely physical system of these little stones
link |
01:42:20.460
rolling around in there
link |
01:42:22.020
and directing where your eyes should go, okay?
link |
01:42:24.920
So you can do this also just by looking up.
link |
01:42:27.140
So let's just say you're sitting in a chair.
link |
01:42:29.200
You're going to look up towards the ceiling
link |
01:42:30.980
and your eyes will just go there.
link |
01:42:32.300
You're doing this eyes open and you look down.
link |
01:42:34.180
Now try doing it really, really slowly.
link |
01:42:41.980
Some people even get motion sick doing this,
link |
01:42:44.260
which if you do, then just stop, okay?
link |
01:42:46.900
So you can do this also to the side,
link |
01:42:49.080
although it works best if you're moving your head
link |
01:42:50.500
from side to side or nodding up and down.
link |
01:42:55.320
So what we're doing here
link |
01:42:56.420
is we're uncoupling these two mechanisms.
link |
01:42:58.300
We're pulling them apart, the visual part
link |
01:43:00.020
and the vestibular part, just to illustrate to you
link |
01:43:02.300
that normally these mechanisms in your inner ear
link |
01:43:06.740
tell your eyes where to go,
link |
01:43:09.000
but as well, your eyes tell your balance system,
link |
01:43:12.540
your vestibular system, how to function.
link |
01:43:15.420
So I'd like you to do a different experiment.
link |
01:43:18.020
I'm not going to do it right now, but basically stand up.
link |
01:43:21.520
If you get the opportunity,
link |
01:43:22.360
you can do this safely wherever you are.
link |
01:43:23.780
You're going to stand up
link |
01:43:25.100
and you're going to look forward about 10, 12 feet.
link |
01:43:27.980
Pick a point on a wall or you can, anywhere that you like.
link |
01:43:31.500
If you're out in public, just do it anyway.
link |
01:43:33.660
Just tell them you're listening to Huberman Lab Podcast
link |
01:43:36.740
and someone's telling you to do it.
link |
01:43:38.900
Anyway, if you don't want to do it, don't do it,
link |
01:43:40.260
but basically do it.
link |
01:43:41.500
Stand on one leg and lift up the other leg.
link |
01:43:45.120
You can bend your knee if you like,
link |
01:43:46.620
and just look off into the distance about 10, 12 feet.
link |
01:43:51.500
If you can do that, if you can stand on one leg,
link |
01:43:53.540
now close your eyes.
link |
01:43:55.780
Chances are you're going to suddenly feel
link |
01:43:58.180
what scientists call postural sway.
link |
01:44:00.360
You're going to start swaying around a lot.
link |
01:44:02.480
It is very hard to balance with your eyes closed.
link |
01:44:06.500
You might think, well, and if you think about that,
link |
01:44:07.820
it's like, why is that?
link |
01:44:09.100
That's crazy.
link |
01:44:10.060
Why would it be that it's hard to balance
link |
01:44:11.900
with your eyes closed?
link |
01:44:12.740
Well, information about the visual world
link |
01:44:15.780
also feeds back onto this vestibular system.
link |
01:44:18.740
So the vestibular system informs your vision
link |
01:44:20.740
and tells you where to move your eyes,
link |
01:44:22.300
and your eyes and their positioning
link |
01:44:24.060
tell your balance system, your vestibular system,
link |
01:44:26.980
how it should function.
link |
01:44:28.660
So there's a really cool way
link |
01:44:31.020
that you can learn to optimize balance.
link |
01:44:33.700
You're not going to try and do this
link |
01:44:34.800
by learning to balance with your eyes closed.
link |
01:44:37.300
What you can do is you can raise one leg,
link |
01:44:41.040
and you can look at a short distance,
link |
01:44:43.000
maybe off to just the distance that your thumb would be
link |
01:44:45.240
if you were to reach your arm out in front of you,
link |
01:44:46.700
although you don't necessarily have to put your thumb
link |
01:44:48.560
in front of you.
link |
01:44:49.400
So maybe just about two feet in front of you.
link |
01:44:51.540
Then while still balancing,
link |
01:44:52.680
you're going to step your vision out a further distance,
link |
01:44:56.220
and then a further distance,
link |
01:44:57.420
and as far as you can possibly see
link |
01:44:58.820
in the environment that you're in.
link |
01:45:00.540
And then you're going to march it back to you.
link |
01:45:03.220
Now, what the literature shows
link |
01:45:05.540
is that this kind of balance training
link |
01:45:07.560
where you incorporate the visual system
link |
01:45:10.580
and extending out and then marching back in,
link |
01:45:14.140
the point at which you direct your visual focus,
link |
01:45:16.900
sends robust information about the relationship
link |
01:45:20.460
between your visual world and your balance system.
link |
01:45:24.100
And of course, the balance system includes
link |
01:45:25.460
not just these hula hoops, these semicircular canals,
link |
01:45:28.540
but they communicate with the cerebellum,
link |
01:45:30.420
the so-called mini brain actually means mini brain
link |
01:45:32.900
in the back of your brain,
link |
01:45:33.900
combines that with visual information
link |
01:45:35.420
and your map of the body surface.
link |
01:45:39.100
That pattern of training is very beneficial
link |
01:45:44.500
for enhancing your ability to balance
link |
01:45:47.300
because the ability to balance is in part
link |
01:45:50.100
the activation of particular postural muscles,
link |
01:45:53.300
but just as much, perhaps even more so,
link |
01:45:57.220
it's about being able to adjust those postural muscles,
link |
01:46:02.340
excuse me, it's about the ability
link |
01:46:03.700
to adjust those postural muscles
link |
01:46:05.620
as you experience changes in your visual world.
link |
01:46:08.900
And one of the most robust ways that you can engage changes
link |
01:46:12.180
in your visual world is through your own movement.
link |
01:46:15.140
And so most people are not trying to balance in place.
link |
01:46:19.280
They're not just trying to stand there
link |
01:46:20.540
like a statue on one leg.
link |
01:46:22.260
Most of what we think about when we think about balance
link |
01:46:24.180
is for sake of sport or dynamic balance,
link |
01:46:26.760
of being able to break ourselves
link |
01:46:28.100
and when we're lunging in one particular direction
link |
01:46:30.820
to stop ourselves, that is,
link |
01:46:32.180
and then to move in another direction
link |
01:46:33.900
or for skateboarding or surfing or cycling
link |
01:46:36.700
or any number of different things, gymnastics.
link |
01:46:39.160
So the visual system is the primary input
link |
01:46:41.940
by which you develop balance,
link |
01:46:43.340
but you can't do it just with the visual system.
link |
01:46:46.460
So what I'm recommending is if you're interested
link |
01:46:49.300
in cultivating a sense of balance,
link |
01:46:50.700
understand the relationship between
link |
01:46:52.260
these semicircular canals.
link |
01:46:54.440
Understand that they are both driving eye movements
link |
01:46:58.820
and they are driven by eye movements.
link |
01:47:01.100
It's a reciprocal relationship.
link |
01:47:03.060
And then even just two or three minutes a day
link |
01:47:06.500
or every once in a while, even three times a week,
link |
01:47:10.020
maybe five minutes, maybe 10 minutes, you pick.
link |
01:47:13.500
But if you want to enhance balance,
link |
01:47:14.820
you have to combine changes in your visual environment
link |
01:47:18.860
with a static posture, standing on one leg
link |
01:47:21.180
and shifting your visual environment
link |
01:47:23.600
or static visual view, looking at one thing
link |
01:47:29.420
and changing your body posture, okay?
link |
01:47:32.640
So those two things we now know
link |
01:47:34.160
from the scientific literature combine
link |
01:47:37.040
in order to give an enhanced sense of balance.
link |
01:47:39.020
And there's a really nice paper that was published in 2015
link |
01:47:42.620
called Effects of Balance Training on Balance Performance.
link |
01:47:45.540
This was in Healthy Adults.
link |
01:47:47.940
It's a systematic review and a meta-analysis.
link |
01:47:51.000
A meta-analysis is when you combine a lot of literature
link |
01:47:53.480
from a lot of different papers
link |
01:47:54.660
and extract the really robust
link |
01:47:56.640
and the less robust statistical effects.
link |
01:47:59.300
So it's a really nice paper as well.
link |
01:48:02.080
There are some papers out there, for instance,
link |
01:48:04.740
a comparison of static balance
link |
01:48:06.760
and the role of vision in the elite athletes.
link |
01:48:09.340
This is essentially the paper that I've extracted
link |
01:48:11.540
most of the information that I just gave you from.
link |
01:48:14.540
And that paper, and there are some others as well,
link |
01:48:18.220
but basically I distilled them down
link |
01:48:19.820
into their core components.
link |
01:48:20.820
The core components are move your vision around
link |
01:48:23.900
while staying static still,
link |
01:48:26.860
but in a balanced position like standing on one leg.
link |
01:48:29.340
Could be something more complicated
link |
01:48:30.620
if you're somebody who can do more complicated movements.
link |
01:48:34.680
Unilateral movements seem to be important.
link |
01:48:37.160
So standing on one leg as opposed to both, right?
link |
01:48:40.400
Or trying to generate some tilt
link |
01:48:43.420
is another way to go about it or imbalance,
link |
01:48:45.420
meaning one limb asymmetrically being activated
link |
01:48:48.100
compared to the other limb.
link |
01:48:49.800
And then the other way to encourage or to cultivate
link |
01:48:54.420
and build up this vestibular system
link |
01:48:56.700
and your sense of balance
link |
01:48:57.960
actually involves movement itself, acceleration.
link |
01:49:01.420
So that's what we're going to talk about now.
link |
01:49:03.440
So up until now, I've been talking about balance
link |
01:49:05.320
only in the static sense,
link |
01:49:06.700
like standing on one leg, for instance,
link |
01:49:08.440
but that's a very artificial situation.
link |
01:49:11.060
Even though you can train balance that way,
link |
01:49:13.160
most people who want to enhance their sense of balance
link |
01:49:15.820
for sport or dance or some other endeavor
link |
01:49:18.780
want to engage balance in a dynamic way,
link |
01:49:21.500
meaning moving through lots of different planes of movement,
link |
01:49:24.180
maybe even sometimes while squatting down low
link |
01:49:26.860
or jumping and landing
link |
01:49:28.160
or making trajectories that are different angles.
link |
01:49:32.700
For that, we need to consider that the vestibular system
link |
01:49:36.860
also cares about acceleration.
link |
01:49:39.540
So it cares about head position,
link |
01:49:40.960
it cares about eye position
link |
01:49:42.620
and where the eyes are and where you're looking,
link |
01:49:44.700
but it also cares about what direction you're moving
link |
01:49:47.860
and how fast.
link |
01:49:49.380
And one of the best things that you can do
link |
01:49:52.420
to enhance your sense of balance
link |
01:49:54.860
is to start to bring together your visual system,
link |
01:49:59.400
the semicircular canals of the inner ear,
link |
01:50:02.300
and what we call linear acceleration.
link |
01:50:05.060
So if I move forward in space rigidly upright,
link |
01:50:08.880
it's a vastly different situation
link |
01:50:11.000
than if I'm leaning to the side.
link |
01:50:14.600
One of the best ways to cultivate a better sense of balance,
link |
01:50:19.160
literally, within the sense organs and the neurons
link |
01:50:23.000
and the biology of the brain
link |
01:50:25.900
is to get into modes where we are accelerating forward,
link |
01:50:30.660
typically it's forward,
link |
01:50:31.940
while also tilted with respect to gravity.
link |
01:50:35.640
Now, this would be the carve on a skateboard
link |
01:50:38.500
or on a surfboard or a snowboard.
link |
01:50:40.720
This would be the taking a corner on a bike
link |
01:50:44.240
while being able to lean safely, of course,
link |
01:50:46.780
lean into the turn so that your head is actually tilted
link |
01:50:50.480
with respect to the earth.
link |
01:50:52.840
So anytime that we are rigidly upright,
link |
01:50:55.740
we aren't really exercising the vestibular system and balance.
link |
01:51:00.420
This is why, you know, you see people in the gym on these,
link |
01:51:03.120
what are those Bonshi balls, Bonshi balls?
link |
01:51:05.260
Bonshi ball's the one that the guys roll in the park, right?
link |
01:51:07.460
Bonshi balls where they're balancing back and forth.
link |
01:51:09.480
That will work the small stabilizing muscles.
link |
01:51:11.920
But what I'm talking about is getting into modes
link |
01:51:13.840
where you actually tilt the body and the head
link |
01:51:17.240
with respect to earth.
link |
01:51:20.540
What I mean is with respect to earth's gravitational pull.
link |
01:51:24.080
Now, the cerebellum is a very interesting structure
link |
01:51:27.320
because not only is it involved in balance,
link |
01:51:29.800
but it's also involved in skill learning
link |
01:51:33.040
and in generating timing of movements.
link |
01:51:35.940
It's a fascinating structure deserving of an entire episode
link |
01:51:39.020
or several episodes all on its own.
link |
01:51:41.760
But some of the outputs of the cerebellum,
link |
01:51:44.960
meaning the neurons in the cell cerebellum get inputs,
link |
01:51:47.800
but they also send information out.
link |
01:51:50.480
The outputs of the cerebellum are strongly linked
link |
01:51:53.400
to areas of the brain that release neuromodulators
link |
01:51:56.520
that make us feel really good,
link |
01:51:58.880
in particular, serotonin and dopamine.
link |
01:52:01.320
And this is an early emerging subfield within neuroscience,
link |
01:52:05.020
but a lot of what are called the non-motor outputs
link |
01:52:07.880
of the cerebellum have a profound influence,
link |
01:52:11.200
not just on our ability to learn how to balance better,
link |
01:52:15.640
but also how we feel overall.
link |
01:52:18.420
So for you exercisers out there,
link |
01:52:20.740
I do hope people are getting
link |
01:52:22.200
regular healthy amounts of exercise.
link |
01:52:23.900
We've talked about what that means in previous episodes.
link |
01:52:26.560
So at least 150 minutes a week of endurance work,
link |
01:52:29.080
some strength training, a minimum of five sets per body part
link |
01:52:31.800
to maintain musculature.
link |
01:52:33.320
Even if you don't want to grow muscles,
link |
01:52:34.880
you want to do that in order to maintain
link |
01:52:36.440
healthy strength and bones, et cetera.
link |
01:52:39.440
If you're doing that,
link |
01:52:40.380
but you're only doing things like curls in the gym,
link |
01:52:43.860
squats in the gym, riding the Peloton,
link |
01:52:46.000
or even if you're outside running
link |
01:52:47.440
and you're getting forward acceleration,
link |
01:52:49.440
but you're never actually getting tilted, right?
link |
01:52:51.240
You're never actually getting tilted
link |
01:52:52.800
with respect to Earth's gravitational pull.
link |
01:52:56.400
You're not really exercising
link |
01:52:58.560
and getting the most out of your nervous system.
link |
01:53:03.200
Activation of the cerebellum in this way of being tilted
link |
01:53:07.840
or the head being tilted and the body being tilted
link |
01:53:09.880
while in acceleration, typically forward acceleration,
link |
01:53:12.960
but sometimes side to side,
link |
01:53:14.920
has a profound and positive effect
link |
01:53:18.280
on our sense of mood and wellbeing.
link |
01:53:20.240
And as I talked about in a previous episode,
link |
01:53:22.840
it can also enhance our ability to learn information
link |
01:53:26.180
in the period after generating those tilts
link |
01:53:29.600
and the acceleration.
link |
01:53:31.360
And that's because the cerebellum has these outputs
link |
01:53:33.800
to these areas of the brain
link |
01:53:34.840
that release these neuromodulators
link |
01:53:36.460
like serotonin and dopamine,
link |
01:53:38.760
and they make us feel really good.
link |
01:53:41.440
I think this is one of the reasons why growing up,
link |
01:53:44.160
I had some friends, some of whom might've been
link |
01:53:47.580
the world heavyweight champions of laziness
link |
01:53:50.240
for essentially everything,
link |
01:53:52.160
except they would wake up at 4.30 in the morning
link |
01:53:55.080
to go surf.
link |
01:53:56.320
They would drive, they would get up so early to go surf.
link |
01:54:00.080
It's not just surfers, and some surfers, by the way,
link |
01:54:02.400
I should point out are not lazy humans.
link |
01:54:04.680
They do a lot of other things,
link |
01:54:06.040
but I knew people that couldn't be motivated to do anything,
link |
01:54:08.940
but they were highly driven to get into these experiences
link |
01:54:13.120
of forward acceleration while tilted
link |
01:54:15.200
with respect to gravity.
link |
01:54:16.580
Likewise with snowboarding or skiing or cycling,
link |
01:54:21.120
those modes of exercise seem to have an outsized effect
link |
01:54:25.800
both on our wellbeing and our ability
link |
01:54:28.380
to translate the vestibular balance
link |
01:54:31.860
that we achieve in those endeavors
link |
01:54:33.640
to our ability to balance while doing other things.
link |
01:54:37.120
So, and I don't mean psychological balance necessarily,
link |
01:54:39.680
I mean physical balance.
link |
01:54:41.540
So for those of you that don't think of yourselves
link |
01:54:43.440
as very coordinated or with very good balance,
link |
01:54:46.960
getting into these modes of acceleration,
link |
01:54:49.000
forward movement or lateral movement while getting tilted,
link |
01:54:51.720
even if you have to do it slowly, could be beneficial,
link |
01:54:54.220
I do believe, and the scientific literature points
link |
01:54:56.560
to the fact that it will be beneficial
link |
01:54:58.700
for cultivating better sense of physical balance.
link |
01:55:02.500
It can really build up the circuits
link |
01:55:04.440
of this vestibular system.
link |
01:55:06.280
And then of course the feel-good components
link |
01:55:08.800
of acceleration while tilted
link |
01:55:11.320
or while getting the head into different orientations
link |
01:55:13.620
relative to gravity.
link |
01:55:16.200
Well, that's the explanation for roller coasters.
link |
01:55:18.740
Some people hate roller coasters,
link |
01:55:20.040
they make them feel nauseous.
link |
01:55:21.960
Many people love roller coasters.
link |
01:55:24.840
And one of the reasons they love roller coasters
link |
01:55:26.720
is because of the way that when you get the body,
link |
01:55:30.280
even if you're not generating the movement,
link |
01:55:31.880
you get the body into forward acceleration
link |
01:55:34.000
and you're going upside down and tilted to the side
link |
01:55:36.480
as the tracks go from side to side and tilt, et cetera,
link |
01:55:39.240
you're getting activation of these deeper brain nuclei
link |
01:55:42.800
that trigger the release of neuromodulators
link |
01:55:45.280
that just make us feel really, really good.
link |
01:55:47.200
In fact, some people get a long arc, a long duration
link |
01:55:50.360
kind of buzz from having gone through those experiences.
link |
01:55:53.080
Some people who hate roller coasters
link |
01:55:54.760
are probably getting nauseous just hearing about that.
link |
01:55:57.960
So I encourage people to get into modes of acceleration
link |
01:56:01.740
while tilted every once in a while,
link |
01:56:03.280
provided you can do it safely.
link |
01:56:04.660
It's an immensely powerful way to build up your skills
link |
01:56:08.560
in the realm of balance.
link |
01:56:10.120
And it's also, for most people, very, very pleasing.
link |
01:56:14.020
It feels really good because of the chemical relationship
link |
01:56:16.600
between forward acceleration and head tilt and body tilt.
link |
01:56:21.200
Now, speaking of feeling nauseous, some people suffer
link |
01:56:25.640
from vertigo, some people feel dizzy,
link |
01:56:28.840
some people get lightheaded.
link |
01:56:31.360
An important question to ask yourself always
link |
01:56:33.900
if you're feeling quote unquote dizzy or lightheaded is,
link |
01:56:36.940
are you dizzy or are you lightheaded?
link |
01:56:39.620
Now, we're not going to diagnose anything here
link |
01:56:41.720
because there's just no way we can do that.
link |
01:56:43.420
This is essentially me shouting into a tunnel
link |
01:56:45.300
so we don't know what's going on
link |
01:56:46.800
with each and every one of you.
link |
01:56:48.240
But if ever you feel that your world is spinning,
link |
01:56:53.140
but that you can focus on your thumb, for instance,
link |
01:56:56.520
but the rest of the world is spinning
link |
01:56:58.560
and your thumb is stationary, that's called being dizzy.
link |
01:57:02.760
Now, if you feel like you're falling
link |
01:57:05.160
or that you feel like you need to get down onto the ground
link |
01:57:07.960
because you feel lightheaded, that's being lightheaded.
link |
01:57:12.080
And oftentimes with language, we don't distinguish
link |
01:57:14.320
between being dizzy and being lightheaded.
link |
01:57:16.580
Now, there are a lot of ways that dizziness
link |
01:57:19.160
and lightheadedness can occur.
link |
01:57:21.000
And I don't even want to begin to guess
link |
01:57:22.440
at the number of different things and ways
link |
01:57:24.820
that it could happen for those of you that suffer from it
link |
01:57:26.740
because it could be any number of them.
link |
01:57:28.920
But oftentimes if people are lightheaded,
link |
01:57:31.920
yes, it could be low blood sugar.
link |
01:57:33.200
It could also be that you're dehydrated.
link |
01:57:35.820
It could also be that you are low in electrolytes.
link |
01:57:38.720
We talked about this in a previous episode,
link |
01:57:40.680
but we will talk about it more in a future episode.
link |
01:57:43.280
Many people have too little sodium in their system, salt,
link |
01:57:46.920
and that's why they feel lightheaded.
link |
01:57:49.360
I have family members who for years
link |
01:57:51.240
thought they had disrupted blood sugar.
link |
01:57:53.800
They would get shaky, jittery, lightheaded,
link |
01:57:56.960
feel like they were nauseous, et cetera.
link |
01:57:59.160
And simply the addition of a little sea salt
link |
01:58:02.160
to their water remedied the problem entirely.
link |
01:58:04.320
I don't think it's going to remedy every issue
link |
01:58:06.680
of lightheadedness out there by any stretch,
link |
01:58:08.940
but just the addition of salt in this particular case
link |
01:58:12.040
helped the person and they are not alone.
link |
01:58:14.600
Many people would think that they have low blood sugar
link |
01:58:16.440
actually are lightheaded because of low electrolytes
link |
01:58:19.640
and because of the way that salt carries water
link |
01:58:22.920
into the system and creates changes in blood volume, et cetera
link |
01:58:26.800
low sodium can often be a source of lightheadedness
link |
01:58:30.480
as can low blood sugar and of course other things as well.
link |
01:58:33.440
Now for dizziness or seasickness,
link |
01:58:36.000
we were all taught that you need to pick a point
link |
01:58:38.880
on the horizon and focus on it.
link |
01:58:41.700
But actually that's not correct.
link |
01:58:45.760
It is true that if you are down in the cabin of a boat
link |
01:58:49.460
or you're on the lower deck
link |
01:58:51.140
and all you can see are things up close to you
link |
01:58:54.760
that getting sloshed around like so
link |
01:58:57.360
or the boat going up and down like so,
link |
01:58:59.840
I think I'm getting a little seasick
link |
01:59:00.860
even as I do this and I describe it.
link |
01:59:06.200
Focusing on things close to you can be problematic.
link |
01:59:09.120
And in that case, the advice to go up on deck
link |
01:59:12.400
and get fresh air and to look off into the horizon,
link |
01:59:15.900
that part is correct.
link |
01:59:17.600
But focusing your eyes on a particular location
link |
01:59:20.000
on the horizon is effectively like trying
link |
01:59:23.080
to move very slowly as I had you do before
link |
01:59:26.260
where you're trying to move your head very slowly
link |
01:59:28.240
while fixating on one location.
link |
01:59:30.500
Your eyes and your balance system
link |
01:59:31.920
were designed to move together.
link |
01:59:33.920
So really what you want to do is allow your visual system
link |
01:59:36.800
to track with your vestibular system.
link |
01:59:40.220
This is why sitting in the back of an Uber or a taxi
link |
01:59:43.420
and being on your phone
link |
01:59:45.300
can make you suddenly feel very nauseous.
link |
01:59:47.700
Sometimes the cabs, particularly in New York City,
link |
01:59:49.700
they have a lot of occluders.
link |
01:59:51.000
They have a lot of stuff blocking your field of view.
link |
01:59:53.160
There's usually a little portal out
link |
01:59:54.860
that where you can see out to the front windshield,
link |
01:59:59.240
but there's all this stuff now.
link |
02:00:00.280
Television's in the back seat
link |
02:00:01.800
and you're watching that television and the cab is moving.
link |
02:00:04.120
You're in linear acceleration
link |
02:00:06.300
and sometimes you're taking corners, you're braking.
link |
02:00:09.000
So then your vestibular system has to adjust to that.
link |
02:00:11.680
If you're looking at your phone or a book,
link |
02:00:14.520
or even if you're talking to somebody,
link |
02:00:16.960
actually, I'm starting to feel a little nauseous.
link |
02:00:19.140
I promise I'm not going to finish this episode
link |
02:00:20.880
by vomiting at the end, at least not here.
link |
02:00:23.860
But what can happen is that you're uncoupling
link |
02:00:28.120
the visual information from your motion,
link |
02:00:31.100
from your vestibular information.
link |
02:00:32.860
You want those to be coupled.
link |
02:00:34.160
This is why a lot of people have to drive.
link |
02:00:36.920
They can't be in the passenger seat
link |
02:00:38.400
because when you drive,
link |
02:00:39.240
you also get what's called proprioceptive feedback.
link |
02:00:41.480
Your body is sending signals also to the vestibular system
link |
02:00:44.920
about where you are in space.
link |
02:00:46.880
When you're the passenger,
link |
02:00:48.080
you're just getting jolted around as the person is driving.
link |
02:00:51.120
And if you're looking at your phone, it's even worse.
link |
02:00:53.320
And if you're looking at the occluder
link |
02:00:54.660
between you and the two front seats, that's even worse.
link |
02:00:58.680
So this is why staring out the front windshield is great,
link |
02:01:01.360
but you don't want to fixate, okay?
link |
02:01:03.280
So hopefully I spared a few people
link |
02:01:05.880
and hopefully a few cab drivers
link |
02:01:07.560
of having people get sick in their cars or Ubers.
link |
02:01:11.300
Let your visual system
link |
02:01:12.360
and your vestibular system work together.
link |
02:01:15.320
If appropriate, get into linear acceleration
link |
02:01:18.560
and you'll improve your sense of balance.
link |
02:01:20.800
Once again, we've covered a tremendous amount
link |
02:01:22.760
of information.
link |
02:01:24.300
Now you know how you hear,
link |
02:01:25.940
how you make sense of the sounds in your environment,
link |
02:01:29.560
how those come into your ears
link |
02:01:31.160
and how your brain processes them.
link |
02:01:33.400
In addition, we talked about things
link |
02:01:35.080
like low-level white noise and even binaural beats,
link |
02:01:38.100
which can be used to enhance certain brain states,
link |
02:01:40.740
certain rhythms within the brain and even dopamine release
link |
02:01:44.160
in ways that allow you to learn better.
link |
02:01:46.800
And we talked about the balance system
link |
02:01:48.480
and this incredible relationship
link |
02:01:50.280
between your vestibular apparatus,
link |
02:01:52.940
meaning the portions of your inner ear
link |
02:01:55.160
that are responsible for balance
link |
02:01:56.520
and your visual system and gravity.
link |
02:01:59.560
And you can use those to enhance your learning as well,
link |
02:02:02.780
as well as just to enhance your sense of balance.
link |
02:02:05.640
If you're learning from this podcast,
link |
02:02:07.360
please subscribe on YouTube.
link |
02:02:08.800
That really helps us.
link |
02:02:10.100
In addition, please leave us any comments or feedback
link |
02:02:13.560
or suggestions for future episode content on YouTube
link |
02:02:16.660
in the comment section.
link |
02:02:18.840
If you haven't already subscribed on Apple and Spotify,
link |
02:02:21.240
please do that as well.
link |
02:02:22.800
And on Apple, you have the opportunity
link |
02:02:24.400
to leave us up to a five-star review.
link |
02:02:26.600
At Apple, you can also leave us comments and feedback.
link |
02:02:29.720
During this episode, I mentioned some supplements.
link |
02:02:32.100
We partnered with Thorne
link |
02:02:33.200
because Thorne has the very highest levels of stringency
link |
02:02:35.840
with respect to the quality of their ingredients
link |
02:02:38.280
and accuracy about the amounts of those ingredients
link |
02:02:41.280
contained within their products.
link |
02:02:42.960
If you'd like to see the products that I take from Thorne,
link |
02:02:45.480
you can go to thorne.com slash the letter U slash Huberman.
link |
02:02:50.860
So that's thorne.com slash U slash Huberman
link |
02:02:54.800
to see all the supplements that I take.
link |
02:02:56.760
And if you do that,
link |
02:02:57.600
you can get 20% off any of those supplements
link |
02:03:00.220
or 20% off any of the supplements that Thorne makes.
link |
02:03:03.560
For those of you that might want to support us
link |
02:03:05.100
in other ways, we have a Patreon account.
link |
02:03:07.300
It's patreon.com slash Andrew Huberman.
link |
02:03:10.440
And there you can support our podcast
link |
02:03:12.160
at any level that you like.
link |
02:03:13.840
In addition, if you'd like to support the podcast,
link |
02:03:16.440
please check out our sponsors mentioned
link |
02:03:18.000
at the beginning of the episode.
link |
02:03:19.380
That is absolutely the best way to support us.
link |
02:03:21.860
Last but not least,
link |
02:03:22.760
I'd like to thank you for your time and attention
link |
02:03:25.160
and desire and willingness to learn
link |
02:03:27.280
about vision and balance.
link |
02:03:29.120
And of course, thank you for your interest in science.
link |
02:03:31.880
And as always, thanks for watching.